BMC Public HealthBMC Public HealthBMC Public Health1471-2458BioMed Central2496220440991571471-2458-14-64310.1186/1471-2458-14-643Research ArticleEpidemiologic studies of modifiable factors associated with cognition and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysisBeydounMay A1baydounm@mail.nih.govBeydounHind A2baydouha@evms.eduGamaldoAlyssa A1gamaldoaa@mail.nih.govTeelAlison2TeelAL@evms.eduZondermanAlan B1zondermana@mail.nih.govWangYoufa34youfawan@buffalo.eduLaboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging, NIA/NIH/IRP, 251 Bayview Blvd., Suite 100, Room #: 04B118, Baltimore, MD 21224, USAGraduate program in Public Health, Eastern Virginia Medical School, Norfolk, VA, UKDepartment of Epidemiology and Environmental Health, School of Public Health and Health Professions, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, New York, USAJohn Hopkins Global Center on Childhood Obesity, Where Systems Science Meets Public Health, Department of International Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA201424620141464364311920131352014Copyright © 2014 Beydoun et al.; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.2014Beydoun et al.; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.Background

Cognitive impairment, including dementia, is a major health concern with the increasing aging population. Preventive measures to delay cognitive decline are of utmost importance. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most frequent cause of dementia, increasing in prevalence from <1% below the age of 60 years to >40% above 85 years of age.

Methods

We systematically reviewed selected modifiable factors such as education, smoking, alcohol, physical activity, caffeine, antioxidants, homocysteine (Hcy), n-3 fatty acids that were studied in relation to various cognitive health outcomes, including incident AD. We searched MEDLINE for published literature (January 1990 through October 2012), including cross-sectional and cohort studies (sample sizes > 300). Analyses compared study finding consistency across factors, study designs and study-level characteristics. Selecting studies of incident AD, our meta-analysis estimated pooled risk ratios (RR), population attributable risk percent (PAR%) and assessed publication bias.

Results

In total, 247 studies were retrieved for systematic review. Consistency analysis for each risk factor suggested positive findings ranging from ~38.9% for caffeine to ~89% for physical activity. Education also had a significantly higher propensity for “a positive finding” compared to caffeine, smoking and antioxidant-related studies. Meta-analysis of 31 studies with incident AD yielded pooled RR for low education (RR = 1.99; 95% CI: 1.30-3.04), high Hcy (RR = 1.93; 95% CI: 1.50-2.49), and current/ever smoking status (RR = 1.37; 95% CI: 1.23-1.52) while indicating protective effects of higher physical activity and n-3 fatty acids. Estimated PAR% were particularly high for physical activity (PAR% = 31.9; 95% CI: 22.7-41.2) and smoking (PAR%=31.09%; 95% CI: 17.9-44.3). Overall, no significant publication bias was found.

Conclusions

Higher Hcy levels, lower educational attainment, and decreased physical activity were particularly strong predictors of incident AD. Further studies are needed to support other potential modifiable protective factors, such as caffeine.

CognitionDementiaAlzheimer’s diseaseRisk factorNutritionMeta-analysis
Background

Cognitive function refers to those mental processes that are crucial for the conduct of the activities of daily living. Such mental processes include attention, short-term and long-term memory, reasoning, coordination of movement and planning of tasks [1]. The prevalence of brain disorders affecting cognition (such as stroke and dementia) increases steadily in a linear fashion with age [2]. Cognitive impairment is a major health concern affecting loss of independence in daily activities in old age. Thus, special attention should be devoted to its prevention [3].

Dementia is relatively frequent in the elderly population and was shown to affect about 6.4% of European subjects over the age of 65 years [4]. A review of 50 original articles published between 1989 and 2002 using international data showed that prevalence of dementia for the very old group (85 years and over) varied from 16.7% in China [5] to 43% in Germany [6]. This variability was also reflected within separate age groups among the very old, ranging from 9.6% to 32% for the 85–89 age category and from 41% to 58% for the 95+ age group. Incidence varied between 47 and 116.6 per 1000 and a separate meta-analytic study estimated the incidence in that age group (i.e. 85+) to be around 104 per 1000 [7,8].

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most frequent cause of dementia, increasing in prevalence from less than 1% below the age of 60 years to more than 40% above 85 years of age. The initial phase is generally marked by a progressive deterioration of episodic memory. Other impairments may be entirely absent in the beginning or consist of mild disturbances in naming and executive function. When the process advances, impairment spreads to other aspects of memory and other domains of cognition. Despite lack of curative treatment, epidemiological evidence reveals important risk factors for sporadic AD, many of which are non-modifiable (e.g. ApoE ϵ4, age and sex). This highlights the importance for further evaluation of modifiable risk and preventive factors in that these potential factors may not only delay the onset of cognitive decline, but also can be easily treated. Aside from AD, less frequently occurring forms of dementia include vascular dementia (VaD), mixed dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and Parkinson’s disease with dementia (PD-D), diagnostic criteria for which are described in Table 1. The relative prevalence of AD and VaD and the mixed version of both remain debatable. Ott and colleagues [9] estimated that for the very old, the prevalence of AD was 26.8% while that of VaD was 4.4%. However, VaD appears to be more frequent than AD in certain Japanese and Chinese populations [10].

Diagnostic criteria of Alzheimer’s Disease (AD), Vascular Dementia (VaD), Mixed dementia (MD) and other dementias

DiagnosisCriteria
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (NINCDS-ADRDA) Source[11,12]:
Development of multiple cognitive deficits, with both memory impairment and one (or more) of the following cognitive disturbances:
Aphasia (language disturbance)
Apraxia (learned motor skills disturbance)
Agnosia (visuospatial/sensory disturbance)
Executive functioning (foresight, planning, insight anticipation)
Significant impairment in social or occupational functioning, representing a significant decline from a previous level of functioning
 
Other diagnostic criteria: Hachinski Ischemic Score, ICD-10; DSM-IV; ADDTC; updated NINCDS-ADRDA
Vascular Dementia (VaD) (NINDS-AIREN) Source[13]:
Cognitive decline from previous higher level of function in three areas of function including memory.
Evidence of cerebrovascular disease by examination
Evidence of cerebrovascular disease by neuroimaging
Onset either abrupt or within three months of a recognized stroke.
Vascular Dementia (VaD) (Modified Hachinski Ischemia Score: ≥4) Source[14]:
Two-point items
Abrupt onset
History of stroke
Focal neurologic symptoms
One-point items
Stepwise deterioration
Somatic complaints
History of hypertension
Emotional incontinence
 
Other diagnostic criteria: ICD-10; DSM-IV
Mixed Dementias (MDs)
 
Hachinski Ischemic score
Score based on clinical features: ≤4 = AD; ≥7 = VaD; intermediate score of 5 or 6 = MD.
ICD-10
Cases that met criteria for VaD and AD
DSM-IV
Cases with criteria for primary degenerative dementia of the Alzheimer type and clinical or neuroimagery feature of VaD.
ADDTC
Presence of ischemic vascular disease and a second systemic or brain disorder.
NINDS-AIREN
Typical AD associated with clinical and radiological evidence of stroke.
Other Dementias
 
Fronto-Parietal Dementia (FTD) Source[15]:
Behavioral or cognitive deficits manifested by either (1) or (2):
(1) Early and progressive personality change, with problems in modulating behavior; inappropriate responses/activities.
(2) Early and progressive language changes, with problems in language expression, word meaning, severe dysnomia.
Deficits represent a decline from baseline and cause significant impairment in social and occupational functioning.
Course characterized by gradual onset and continuing decline in function.
Other causes (eg, stroke, delirium) are excluded
Gradual onset and progressive cognitive decline.
Dementia with Lewy Bodies (DLB) (Consensus Guidelines for the Clinical Diagnosis for Dementia with Lewy Bodies) Source[16]:
Fluctuating in cognitive performance: Marked variation in cognition or function, or episodic confusion/decreased responsiveness.
 
Visual hallucinations: Usually well formed, unprovoked, benign.
 
Parkinsonism: Can be identical to Parkinson’s Disease (PD), milder or symmetric.
Parkinson’s Disease with Dementia (PD-D) Source[17]:
Bradyphrenia (slowness of thought)
 
Executive impairment
 
Neuropsychiatric symptoms
 Dysphonia

Sources[17,18]:.

Abbreviations: ADDTC: Alzheimer's Disease Diagnostic and Treatment Centers; DSM-IV: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 4th edition; ICD-10: International Classification of Disease, 10th edition; NINCDS-ADRDA: National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke -- the Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association; NINDS-AIREN: National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke--Association Internationale pour la Recherche et l’Enseignement en Neurosciences; PD-D: Parkinson’s disease with dementia.

The present review focuses on selected modifiable risk and protective factors of cognitive impairment, cognitive decline and dementia (including AD), given that they are commonly studied and provide reliable and comparable data. In particular, we focused on the risk and protective factors that could be grouped under three broad categories, namely socioeconomic, behavioral, and nutritional. Consequently, other known risk factors that are modifiable but did not fall under these categories were excluded (e.g. obesity, type 2 diabetes, hypertension, depression, traumatic head injury etc.). In addition, the latter risk factors are potential mediating factors in the causal pathway between our selected factors and cognitive outcomes (e.g. obesity, depression, type 2 diabetes and hypertension may be on the causal pathway between physical activity and cognition; the same for diet→depression, obesity, type 2 diabetes, hypertension→cognition) and thus are more appropriately studied together rather than with their antecedent putative causes. This is the first study to systematically review those selected modifiable risk and protective factors for cognitive health outcomes in cross-sectional and cohort studies while comparing the consistency of association between those factors and across study-level characteristics. It is also among few recent studies to compare the strength of association across those factors in relation to incident AD using a similar approach [19,20]. Our findings could help guide future research and interventions.

MethodsLiterature search

Using MEDLINE, we conducted a systematic review of the literature on cognitive function, decline and dementia focusing on specific risk factors. We considered both original research published between January 1990 and October 2012. We did not include titles prior to 1990 to ensure that diagnostic criteria for dementia and AD were comparable across studies. After an initial search using MESH keywords for risk factors (i.e. education, smoking, physical activity, caffeine/coffee/tea, alcohol, antioxidant/vitamin E, homocysteine and fatty acid) and a title containing the words (cognitive, dementia and Alzheimer), we assessed the retrieved papers for relevance by reading the titles and abstracts. Among those that were selected for review, information was retrieved including study design, contextual setting, sample size, main outcome and key findings.

Final inclusion criteria were: (1) Study sample size > 300. Although this number is arbitrary, it was based on the fact that some study outcomes were relatively rare (e.g. incident AD: <10%) and thus a smaller sample size for a cohort study for example might yield an underpowered study, depending on the distribution of the risk factor. (2) Study design is either cross-sectional or cohort study (thus case-control studies, review articles, commentaries, and basic science papers were excluded); (3) Outcomes include dementia, AD, cognitive function, cognitive decline or cognitive impairment (including MCI)). Although all types of dementia were presented in our description of selected studies, focus was on the more prevalent sub-types including AD and VaD (4) Baseline sample includes general healthy population rather than special groups at risk (e.g. coronary heart disease patients). For the “cognitive decline” and “cognitive function” outcomes, we searched risk factors in the title to expand the range of studies selected beyond those based just on MESH keywords (i.e. “caffeine,” “alcohol” and all other risk factors were also searched in titles when the outcome was “cognitive”). Both cohort and cross-sectional studies that were selected are presented in Table 2. The MEDLINE search and the studies excluded are laid out in Figure 1, showing main reasons for exclusion and final number of studies included for each risk factor. After inclusion of a study, we did not examine cross-references in order to ensure the comparability of the search strategy between risk factors.

Summary of epidemiologic studies of risk and protective factors for cognitive outcomes included in the review

StudyAge/genderYearCountryStudy designSample sizeOutcomesFindings
(1) Education
Hypothesis: Lower education is associated with lower cognitive function or higher rate of cognitive decline or increased risk of dementia(including AD)
[21]
65+/B
1990
China
Cross-sectional
N = 5,055
Prevalent AD and dementia
+
[22]
Mean:58.5/B
1991
Nigeria
Cross-sectional
N = 1,350
Cognitive function
+
[23]
65+/B
1992
France
Cross-sectional
N = 2,792
Cognitive function
+
[24]
68-77/B
1992
Finland
Cross-sectional
N = 403
Cognitive function
+
[25]
65+/B
1993
US
Cohort
N = 4,485
Cognitive decline
+
[26]
75+/B
1994
England
Cohort
N = 1,195
Indicent dementia
0
[27]
65+/B
1994
US
Cohort
N = 10,294
Incident cognitive impairment
+
[28]
55+/B
1995
US
Cohort
N = 3,330
Incident AD and VaD
+(VaD)
[29]
18+/B
1995
US
Cohort
N = 14,883
Indicent dementia
+
[9]
55-106/B
1995
The Netherlands
Cross-sectional
N = 7,528
Prevalent dementia, AD and VaD
+
[30]
65+/B
1996
US
Cross-sectional
N = 2,212
Prevalent dementia and cognitive impairment
+
[31]
68-78/B
1996
Finland
Cross-sectional
N = 403
Cognitive decline
+
[32]
70+/B
1997
Australia
Cohort
N = 652
Cognitive decline
+
[33]
65+/B
1997
US
Cohort
N = 642
Incident AD
+
[34]
50-80/B
1997
Austria
Cross-sectional
N = 1,927
Cognitive function
+
[35]
Mean:75/M
1997
The Netherlands
Cohort
N = 528
Cognitive decline
+
[36]
69-74/M
1997
Sweden
Cross-sectional
N = 504
Cognitive function
+
[37]
55-84/B
1997
The Netherlands
Cohort
N = 5,825
Cognitive function, decline and incident/prevalent dementia
+
[38]
65-84/B
1997
The Netherlands
Cohort
N = 2,063
Incident dementia
+(IQ better predcitor)
[39]
47-68/B
1998
US
Cross-sectional
N = 14,000
Cognitive function
+
[40]
60+/B
1998
Italy
Cross-sectional
N = 495
Prevalent dementia
0
[41]
65+/B
1998
Taiwan
Cross-sectional
N = 2,915
Prevalent dementia, AD and VaD
+(AD)
[42]
18+/B
1999
US
Cohort
N = 1,488
Cognitive decline
+
[43]
65+/B
1999
France
Cohort
N = 3,675
Incident AD
+
[44]
55-106/B
1999
The Netherlands
Cohort
N = 6,827
Incident dementia
+(women)
[45]
85+/B
2000
Sweden
Cohort
N = 494
Cognitive decline and function
+
[46]
75+/B
2001
Sweden
Cohort
N = 1,296
Incident dementia and AD
+
[47]
65+/B
2002
Spain
Cohort
N = 557
Cognitive decline
+
[48]
70+/B
2002
US
Cross-sectional
N = 6,577
Cognitive function
+
[49]
65+/B
2002
Brazil
Cross-sectional
N = 1,656
Prevalent dementia and AD
+
[50]
65+/B
2002
Italy
Cross-sectional
N = 1,016
Prevalent AD and VaD
+
[51]
45-59/M
2002
US
Cross-sectional
N = 1,839
Cognitive function
+
[52]
70-79/W
2003
US
Cohort
N = 19,319
Cognitive function and decline
+
[53]
70-79/B
2005
US
Cohort
N = 4,030
Cognitive decline
+(ApoE-)
[54]
66+/W
2006
US
Cohort
N = 6,314
Cognitive function and decline
+
[55]
Mean age: ~75/B
2006
US
Cohort
N = 2,786
Incident dementia
+(both whites and blacks)
[56]
55+/B
2006
China
Cross-sectional
N = 34,807
Prevalent AD and VaD
+(AD)
[57]
50+/B
2006
China
Cross-sectional
N = 16,095
Prevalent dementia and AD
+
[58]
65+/B
2007
Guam
Cross-sectional
N = 2,789
Prevalent dementia and AD
+
[59]
64-81/B
2007
The Netherlands
Cross-sectional
N = 578
Cognitive function
+
[60]
60-64/B
2009
Australia
Cohort
N = 416
Cognitive decline
0
[61]
30-64/B
2009
US
Cross-sectional
N = 1,345
Cognitive function
+(literacy better predictor)
[62]
65-96/B
2009
Spain
Cross-sectional
N = 1,074
Prevalent dementia
+
[63]
80+/B
2009
UK
Cohort
N = 3,336
Incident dementia
+
[64]
Mean:72/B
2009
US
Cohort
N = 6,000
Cognitive function and decline
+(cognitive function) 0(cognitive decline)
[65]
60+/B
2010
Malaysa
Cross-sectional
N = 2,980
Prevalent dementia
+
[66]
55+/B
2010
India
Cross-sectional
N = 2,466
Prevalent dementia and AD
+
[67]
65+/B
2010
Brazil
Cross-sectional
N = 2,003
Cognitive function
+
[68]
60+/B
2011
Brazil
Cohort
N = 1,461
Cognitive decline
-
[69]
60-98/B
2011
Italy
Cohort
N = 1,270
Incident cognitive impairment
+
[70]
60+/B
2011
Mexico
Cohort
N = 7,000
Prevalent and incident dementia
+
[71]
54-95/B
2011
US
Cohort
N = 1,014
Cognitive decline
0
Study
Age/gender
Year
Country
Design
Sample size
Outcome
Finding
(2) Behavioral
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
(2.1.) Smoking
Hypothesis: Current or ever smoking status is associated with lower cognitive function or higher rate of cognitive decline or increased risk of dementia(including AD)
[72]
65+/B
1993
US
Cohort
N = 1,201
Cognitive decline
0
[73]
65+/B
1994
France
Cross-sectional
N = 3,770
Prevalent AD, cognitive impairment
0
[74]
74+/B
1996
US
Cohort
N = 647
Cognitive function
0
[75]
Mean:58.6/M
1997
US
Cohort
N = 3,429
Cognitive impairment
+
[36]
69-74/M
1997
Sweden
cross-sectional
N = 504
Cognitive function
+
[76]
75+/B
1998
Australia
Cohort
N = 327
Incident dementia and AD
0
[77]
adults/B
1998
US
Cohort
N = 1,469
Cognitive function
0
[78]
55+/B
1998
US
Cohort
N = 6,870
Incident dementia and AD
+(ApoE4)
[79]
56-69/M
1999
US
Cross-sectional
N = 569
Cognitive impairment
+(ApoE4)
[80]
45-59/M
1999
UK
Cross-sectional
N = 1,870
Cognitive function
0
[81]
65+/B
2000
UK
Cohort
N = 889
Cognitive Impairment
+
[82]
Mean: 81/M
2000
UK
Cohort
N = 34,439
Definite or probable AD
0
[83]
45-70/B
2002
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 1,927
Cognitive change
+
[84]
65+/B
2003
Taiwan
Cohort
N = 798
Cognitive decline
0
[85]
43-53/B
2003
UK
Cohort
N = 3,035
Cognitive decline
+
[86]
Mean:78/M
2003
US
Cohort
N = 3,734
Incident AD
+
[87]
60+/B
2003
China
Cross-sectional
N = 3,012
Cognitive impairment
+
[88]
60+/B
2004
China
Cohort
N = 2,820
Incident dementia and AD
+
[89]
65+/B
2004
European cohorts
Cohort
N = 17,610
Cognitive decline
+
[90]
65-84/B
2004
Italy
Cohort
N = 5,632
Mild cognitive impairment
0
[91]
40-80/M
2004
The Netherlands
Cross-sectional
N = 900
Cognitive function
0
[92]
Mean:75/B
2005
US
Cohort
N = 791
Cognitive function and decline
+(75 + and ApoE4)
[93]
40-44/B
2005
US
Cohort
N = 8,845
Incident dementia
+
[94]
50+/B
2006
UK
Cohort
N = 2,000
Cognitive function
+
[95]
55+/B
2007
The Netherlands
Cohort
N = 6,868
Incident dementia and AD
+
[96]
43-70/B
2008
The Netherlands
Cohort
N = 1,964
Cognitive decline
+
[97]
35-55/B
2008
France
Cohort
N = 4,659
Cognitive function
+(memory)
[98]
46-70/B
2009
US
Cohort
N = 11,151
Incident dementia
+
[99]
65+/B
2009
US
Cohort
N = 1,557
Cognitive decline
+
[100]
90-108/B
2009
China
Cross-sectional
N = 681
Cognitive impairment
+(men)
[63]
Mean:83.5/B
2009
UK
Cohort
N = 3,336
Incident dementia
0
[101]
65-79/B
2010
Finland
Cohort
N = 1,449
Incident dementia and AD
+
[102]
Mean:71.8/B
2010
Taiwan
Cohort
N = 1,436
Incident cognitive impairment
-
[103]
50y/M
2011
Sweden
Cohort
N = 2,268
Incident dementia and AD
+(non-AD)
[104]
Mean:60.1/B
2011
Finland
Cohort
N = 21,123
Incident dementia and AD
+
[105]
44-69/B
2012
UK
Cohort
N = 7,236
Cognitive decline
+(men)
Study
Age/gender
Year
Country
Design
Sample size
Outcome
Finding
(2.2.) Alcohol
Hypothesis: Moderate alcohol consumption is protective against poorer cognitive function, higher rate of cognitive decline and dementia
[106]
65+/B
1996
US
Cross-sectional
N = 2,040
Cognitive function
+(J-shaped)
[107]
59-71/B
1997
France
Cross-sectional
N = 1,389
Cognitive function
+ (women)
[76]
75+/B
1998
Australia
Cohort
N = 327
Incident dementia and AD
0
[77]
40-80/B
1998
US
Cohort
N = 1469
Cognitive function
0
[108]
55-88/B
1999
USA
Cohort
N = 1786
Cognitive function
+(U-shaped)
[109]
65+/B
2001
US
Cross-sectional
N = 1,836
Cognitive function
+(U-shaped for men, linear for women)
[110]
Mean:70/B
2001
Italy
Cross-sectional
N = 15,807
Cognitive impairment
+(U-shaped)
[83]
45-70/B
2002
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 1,927
Cognitive change
+(women > men) (J-shaped)
[111]
18+/B
2000
US
Cohort
N = 1,448
Cognitive decline
+(women > men) (U-shaped)
[112]
53/B
2003
US
Cross-sectional
N = 10,317
Cognitive function
0
[87]
60+/B
2003
China
Cohort
N = 3,012
Cognitive impairment
-
[113]
65-79/B
2004
Finland
Cohort
N = 1,464
Cognitive function
+(U-shaped) - (ApoE4+)
[114]
65+/B
2004
US
Cohort
N = 4,417
Cognitive function
+(current drinker vs. former or abstainer)
[115]
35-55/B
2004
UK
Cohort
N = 10,308
Cognitive function
+(linear, some cognitive domains)
[116]
65+/B
2005
US
cohort
N = 1,624
Cognitive function
+(current drinker vs. former or abstainer)
[117]
Mean:74/B
2005
US
Cohort
N = 1,098
Cognitive function and decline
+(J-shaped)
[118]
43-53/B
2005
UK
Cohort
N = 1,764
Cognitive decline
Linear + (slower memory decline: men) -(faster psychomotor speed decline: women)
[119]
20-24,40-44,60-64/B
 
Australia
Cross-sectional
N = 7,485
Cognitive function
J-shaped + (light drinkers vs. abstainers)
[120]
70-81/W
2005
US
Cohort
N = 11,102
Cognitive function and decline
+(J-shaped) (cognitive decline)
[121]
65-89/M
2006
US
Cross-sectional
N = 760
Cognitive function
+(linear, J-shaped)
[122]
40+/B
2006
US
Cohort
N = 1,428
Cognitive decline
+(linear)
[123]
65-79/B
2006
Finland
Cross-sectional
N = 1,341
Cognitive function
+(linear)
[124]
65-84/B
2007
US
Cohort
N = 1,445
Incident MCI and MCI→ dementia
+(U-shaped)
[125]
50+/B
2010
China
Cohort
N = 30,499
MCI→ dementia
+(J-shaped)
[126]
50+/B
2010
China
Cross-sectional
N = 9,571-28,537
Cognitive function
+(occasional alcohol use vs. none)
[127]
65+/B
2009
China
Cross-sectional
N = 314
Cognitive impairment
+(U-shaped)
[128]
70/B
2011
UK
Cross-sectional
N = 922
Cognitive function
+(linear, verbal memory)
[129]
55+/B
2011
US
Cohort
N = 1,337
Cognitive function
0 -(executive function)
[130]
55+/B
2011
France
Cross-sectional
N = 4,073
Cognitive function
-(high alcohol use, Low SES)
[131]
45+/B
2012
US
Cohort
N = 571
Cognitive decline
+(heavy drinking)
Study
Age/gender
Year
Country
Design
Sample size
Outcome
Finding
(2.3.) Physical activity
Hypothesis: Physical activity is protective against poorer cognitive function, higher rate of cognitive decline and dementia(including AD)
[132]
70+/B
2001
Hong Kong
Cohort
N = 2030
Cognitive impairment
+
[133]
65+/B
2001
Canada
Cohort
N = 4615
Incident cognitive impairment and AD
+
[134]
65-84/M
2001
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 347
Cognitive decline
+(ApoE4+)
[135]
65+/F
2001
US
Cohort
N = 5,925
Cognitive decline
+
[136]
75+/B
2003
US
Cohort
N = 469
Incident dementias (AD, VaD and others)
+
[137]
71-93/M
2004
US
Cohort
N = 2257
Incident dementia and AD
+
[138]
65+/B
2004
US
Cohort
N = 1146
Cognitive decline
+
[139]
80+/M
2004
European countries
Cohort
N = 295
Cognitive decline
+
[140]
70-81/W
2004
US
Cohort
N = 18766
Cognitive decline
+
[141]
65+/M
2005
US
Cohort
N = 3375
Incident dementia and AD
+(ApoE4-)
[142]
65-79/B
2005
Sweden
Cohort
N = 1449
Incident dementia and AD
+
[143]
65+/B
2005
US
Cohort
N = 4055
Cognitive decline
-
[144]
75+/B
2006
Sweden
Cohort
N = 776
Incident dementia
+
[145]
65+/B
2006
US
Cohort
N = 1740
Incident dementia and AD
+
[146]
65+/W
2010
US
Cross-sectional
N = 9344
Cognitive impairment
+
[147]
60+/B
2008
Greece
Cohort
N = 732
Cognitive impairment
+
[148]
71-92/M
2008
US
Cohort
N = 2263
Dementia
+
[149]
70+/B
2009
Italy
Cross-sectional
N = 668
Cognitive decline
+
[100]
90-108/B
2009
China
Cross-sectional
N = 681
Cognitive impairment
+
[150]
65+/B
2009
US
Cohort
N = 1880
Incident AD
+
[151]
70-79/B
2009
US
Cohort
N = 2509
Cognitive function and decline
+
[152]
Mean:51y/B
2010
Iceland
Cohort
N = 4945
Cognitive function and dementia
+
[153]
55+/B
2010
Germany
Cohort
N = 3903
Incident cognitive impairment
+
[154]
60+/B
2010
US
Cohort
N = 5903
Cognitive function
+
[155]
65+/W
2010
US
Cross-sectional
N = 9344
Cognitive function and impairment
+
[156]
Mean:82/B
2012
US
Cohort
N = 716
AD Cognitive decline
+
[157]
40-84/B
2012
US
Cohort
N = 405 (40–59 years) N = 342 (60–84 years)
Cognitive function
+
[158]
65+/B
2012
US
Cohort
N = 2491
Incident dementia & AD
+
Study
Age/gender
Year
Country
Design
Sample size
Outcome
Finding
(3) Nutritional
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
(3.1) Caffeine(coffee or tea)
Hypothesis: Caffeine consumption is protective against poorer cognitive function, higher rate of cognitive decline and dementia
[159]
18+/B
1993
UK
Cross-sectional
N = 9,003
Cognitive function
+(caffeine)
[160]
Mean: 73/B
2002
US
Cross-sectional
N = 1,528
Cognitive function
0(coffee)
[161]
24-81/B
2003
The Netherlands
Cohort
N = 1,376
Cognitive change
0(caffeine)
[162]
70+/B
2006
Japan
Cross-sectional
N = 1,003
Cognitive impairment
+(green tea)
[163]
Mean ~ 75/M
2007
Finland, the Netherlands and Italy
Cohort
N = 667
Cognitive decline
+(coffee, J-shaped)
[164]
55+/B
2008
Singapore
Cohort
N = 1,438
Cognitive impairment and decline
+(tea)
[165]
65-79/B
2009
Finland
Cohort
N = 1,409
Incident dementia and AD
+(coffee), 0(tea)
[100]
90+/B
2009
China
Cross-sectional
N = 681
Cognitive impairment
+(tea, men)
[166]
65+/B
2009
Finland
Cohort
N = 2,606
Cognitive function, incident dementia and MCI
0(coffee)
[167]
70-74/B
2009
Norway
Cross-sectiona
N = 2,031
Cognitive impairment
+(tea)
[168]
17-92/B
2009
UK
Cross-sectional
N = 3,223
Cognitive function
0(caffeine)
[169]
70/B
2010
UK
Cohort
N = 923
Cognitive function
+(coffee); −(tea)
[170]
55+/B
2010
Singapore
Cross-sectional
N = 716
Cognitive function
+(tea)
[171]
65+/B
2010
France
Cohort
N = 641
Cognitive decline
+(caffeine, women)
[172]
65+/B
2010
Portugal
Cohort
N = 648
Cognitive decline
+(caffeine, women)
[173]
65+/B
2011
US
Cohort
N = 4,809
Cognitive decline
+(caffeine, women)
[174]
Mean:54/M
2011
US
Cohort
N = 3,494
Incident dementia and cognitive impairment
0(caffeine)
[175]
Mean:91.4/B
2012
Singapore
Cohort
N = 7,139
Cognitive change
+(tea)
Study
Age/gender
Year
Country
Design
Sample size
Outcome
Finding
(3.2) Antioxidants/Vitamin E
Hypothesis: Antioxidants, including vitamin E, are protective against poorer cognitive function, higher rate of cognitive decline and dementia(including AD)
[176]
55-95/B
1996
Netherlands
cohort
N = 5,182
Cognitive function
+
[177]
66-97/B
1998
US
Cohort
N = 1,059
Cognitive function
0
[178]
65+/B
1998
US
Cohort
N = 633
Incident AD
+
[179]
5075/B
1998
Austria
Cross-sectional
N = 1,769
Cognitive performance
+(Vit. E)
[180]
71-93/M
2000
Hawaii
Cohort
N = 3,385
Incident AD, VaD, MD and OD
+(VaD)
[181]
48-67/B
2000
US
Cross-sectional
N = 12,187
Cognitive performance
0
[182]
55+/B
2002
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 5,395
Incident AD
+
[183]
65+/B
2002
US
Cohort
N = 815
Incident AD
+(Vit. E, ApoE4)
[184]
65-102/B
2002
US
Cohort
N = 2,889
Cognitive decline
+
[185]
65+/B
2003
US
Cohort
N = 2,969
Incident dementia Incident AD
0
[186]
70-79/W
2003
US
Cohort
N = 14,968
Cognitive function
+(Vit. E)
[187]
65+/B
2003
US
Cohort
N = 980
Incident AD
0
[188]
45-68/M
2004
US
Cohort
N = 2,459
Incident dementia and AD
0
[189]
65+/B
2004
US
Cohort
N = 4,740
Incident and prevalent AD
+
[190]
65+/B
2005
Italy
Cross-sectional
N = 1,033
Prevalent dementia and cognitive impairment
+
[191]
55+/B
2005
Netherlands
Cross-sectional
N = 3,717
Prevalent AD
0
[192]
65-105/B
2005
US
Cohort
N = 616
Incident Dementia Incident AD
0
[193]
65+/B
2005
Canada
Cohort
N = 894
Cognitive decline Dementia
+
[194]
65+/B
2005
US
Cohort
N = 3,718
Incident AD Cognitive function
+
[195]
Mean:73.5/B
2007
France
Cross-sectional
N = 589
Cognitive function
+
[196]
60+/W
2007
US
Cohort
N = 526
Cognitive impairment
+(Vit. E)
[197]
65+/B
2007
US
Cohort
N = 3,831
Cognitive function
+
[198]
65+/B
2008
US
Cohort
N = 3,376
Cognitive function
+
[199]
65+/B
2008
US
Cohort
N = 2,969
Incident Dementia Incident AD
0
[200]
65+/B
2008
Italy
Cohort
N = 761
Cognitive impairment
+(Vit. E Sub-type)
[201]
70+/W
2010
US
Cohort
N = 16,010
Cognitive function & decline
+(cognitive function)
[202]
70/B
2011
UK
Cross-sectional
N = 882
Cognitive function
0
Study
Age/gender
Year
Country
Design
Sample size
Outcome
Finding
(3.3) Homocysteine
Hypothesis: Homocysteine is a risk factor for poorer cognitive function, higher rate of cognitive decline and dementia (including AD)
[203]
55+/B
1999
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 702
Cognitive function and decline
0
[204]
60+/B
2002
UK
Cross-sectional
N = 391
Cognitive function
+
[205]
55+/B
2002
The Netherlands
Cross-sectional
N = 1,077
Cognitive function
+
[206]
Mean:76/B
2002
US
Cohort
N = 1,092
Incident AD
+
[207]
60+/B
2003
US
Cross-sectional
N = 1,789
Global cognitive function
+
[208]
Mean:73/B
2003
Italy
Cross-sectional
N = 650
Cognitive function
+
[209]
65+/B
2004
US
Cohort
N = 679
Incident and prevalent AD
0
[210]
Mean:72/B
2005
Turkey
Cohort
N = 1,249
Incident dementia, AD, MCI
0
[211]
60+/B
2005
US
Cross-sectional
N = 1,789
Cognitive impairment and dementia
0
[212]
40-82/B
2005
US
Cross-sectional
N = 2,096
Cognitive function
+(60 + y)
[213]
70-79/B
2005
US
Cohort
N = 499
Cognitive function and decline
+(cognitive function)
[214]
85+/B
2005
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 599
Cognitive impairment and decline
+(with impairment)
[215]
65+/B
2005
Switzerland
Cohort
N = 623
Incident MCI, dementia, AD and VaD
+
[216]
60+/B
2005
US
Cross-sectional
N = 1,789
Cognitive impairment and dementia
+
[217]
Mean:74/B
2005
Italy
Cohort
N = 816
Incident AD
+
[218]
50-70/B
2005
US
Cross-sectional
N = 1,140
Cognitive function
+
[219]
50-85/M
2005
US
Cohort
N = 321
Cognitive decline
+
[220]
Mean:62/B
2006
US
Cross-sectional
N = 812
Cognitive function
+
[221]
55+/B
2006
China
Cross-sectional
N = 451
Cognitive function
+
[222]
Mean:59/B
2006
The Netherlands
Cohort
N = 345
Cognitive function
+
[223]
65+/B
2007
UK
Cohort
N = 1,648
Cognitive decline
+
[224]
60-101/B
2007
US
Cohort
N = 1,779
Incident dementia and MCI
+
[225]
60-85/B
2007
South Korea
Cross-sectional
N = 1,215
Prevalent MCI
+
[226]
26-98/B
2008
US
Cross-sectional
N = 911
Cognitive function
+(ApoE4+)
[227]
65+/B
2008
Korea
Cross-sectional
N = 607
Cognitive function
+
[228]
Mean:72/B
2008
Korea
Cohort
N = 518
Incident dementia and AD
+
[229]
Mean:77/B
2009
US
Cohort
N = 516
Prevalent and incident MCI
0
[230]
38-85/B
2010
Sweden
Cohort
N = 488
Incident dementia
0
[231]
65+/B
2010
The Netherlands
Cohort
N = 1,076
Cognitive decline
+
[232]
Mean:78/W
2011
Germany
Cross-sectional
N = 420
Cognitive function
+
[233]
38-60/W
2011
Sweden
Cohort
N = 1,368
Incident dementia and AD
+
[234]
70-89/M
2012
Australia
Cohort
N = 4,227
Incident dementia
+
[235]
70-89/M
2012
Australia
Cohort
N = 1,778
Incident cognitive impairment
+
Study
Age/gender
Year
Country
Design
Sample size
Outcome
Finding
(3.4) n-3 fatty acids
Hypothesis: n-3 fatty acids are protective against poorer cognitive function, higher rate of cognitive decline and dementia(including AD)
[236]
69-89/M
1997
Netherlands
cohort
N = 476
Cognitive impairment & decline
0
[237]
55+/B
1997
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 5,386
Incident dementia and AD
+
[238]
55+/B
2002
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 5,395
Incident dementia and AD
0
[239]
65-94/B
2003
US
Cohort
N = 815
Incident AD
+
[240]
45-70/B
2004
Netherlands
Cross-sectional
N = 1,613
Cognitive function
+
[241]
65+/B
2005
US
Cohort
N = 3,718
Cognitive decline
0
[242]
65+/B
2007
France
Cohort
N = 8,085
Incident dementia and AD
+(ApoE4-)
[243]
50+/B
2007
US
Cohort
N = 2,251
Cognitive decline
+(hypertensive, Dyslipidemic)
[244]
Mean:76/B
2007
Italy
Cross-sectional
N = 935
Prevalent dementia
+
[245]
50-70/B
2007
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 404-807
Cognitive function and change
+(change)
[246]
50+/B
2008
US
Cohort
N = 2,251-7,814
Cognitive decline
+(hypertensives)
[247]
65-80/B
2008
Finland
Cohort
N = 1,449
MCI and cognitive function
+
[248]
Mean:78/B
2008
France
Cohort
N = 1,214
Incident dementia
+
[249]
65+/B
2009
Multi-national
Cross-sectional
N = 14,960
Prevalent dementia
+
[250]
55+/B
2009
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 5,395
Incident dementia and AD
0
[251]
65+/B
2009
Canada
Cohort
N = 663
Incident dementia or AD
0
[252]
Mean:68/M
2009
Netherlands
Cohort
N = 1,025
Cognitive function
0
[253]
76-82/W
2009
France
Cohort
N = 4,809
Cognitive decline
+
[254]
Mean:75/B
2010
Spain
Cross-sectional
N = 304
Cognitive impairment
+
[255]
35-54/B
2010
US
Cross-sectional
N = 280
Cognitive function
+
[256]
55+/B
2011
Singapore
Cohort
N = 1,475
Cognitive function and decline
+(supplements)
[257]
Mean:~64/B
2011
France
Cohort
N = 3,294
Cognitive impairment
+
[258]65+/B2011FranceCohortN = 1,228Cognitive decline+(ApoE4+; depressed)

+Hypothesized association; − Association against hypothesis; 0: No association.

Abbreviations: AD: Alzheimer’s Disease; ApoE: Apolipoprotein E; B: Both; M: Men; MCI: Mild Cognitive Impairment; MD = Mixed Dementia; OD = Other dementia; UK: United Kingdom; US: United States; VaD: Vascular Dementia; W: Women.

Flowchart of study selection for systematic review and meta-analysis. Notes: MEDLINE searches (1990–2012) included the following: (1) “Risk factor” as MESH term AND “Dementia” in title; (2) “Risk factor” as MESH term AND “Alzheimer” in title; (3) “Risk factor” as MESH term AND “Alzheimer” in title; (4) “Risk factor” as MESH term AND “cognitive” in title; (5) “Risk factor” in title and “cognitive” in title. Given that each search is not mutually exclusive of other searches, there were duplicates which were deleted from the final number of included studies. The following notations are defined follows: N1 = Studies excluded from all searches combined due to small sample size; N2 = Studies excluded from all searches combined due to design being neither cross-sectional nor cohort; N3 = Studies excluded from all searches combined due to being a review or a letter to the editor; N4 = Studies excluded from all searches combined due to lack of relevance to topic or hypothesis; N5 = Studies excluded from all searches combined for other reasons (e.g. special group of people); N6 = Final included studies; N6a = Final included cohort studies; N6b = Final included cross-sectional studies.

Out of a total search of 6,837 titles and abstracts between 1990 and 2012 (range:126 for caffeine to 1,692 for education), 247 published original epidemiologic studies (167 cohort-, 80 cross-sectional studies) were included in our review. A database was built accordingly using Endnote ver. X3 [259]. Each study was summarized in Table 2 by listing the sample characteristics (age, gender, country), study design, sample size and type of outcome. Given the diversity of types of outcomes, a quantitative meta-analysis for all studies with all outcomes was not possible. Thus, a qualitative method to assess overall consistency was conducted. This analysis was mainly based on the hypothesized direction of association and the final conclusion of the study. Thus, main findings based on the pre-set hypothesis was coded (+: supports the hypothesis; 0: no significant finding; −: against the hypothesis). In addition, within +, we coded studies as partially supporting the hypothesis for three main reasons: “some outcomes but not others”, “some exposures but not others”, “some sub-group(s) but not others”. These papers are sorted by risk factor, year of publication and first author’s last name.

Descriptive analysis

In the descriptive part of the analysis, a data point consisted of a study finding within a design/risk factor dyad (e.g. cohort/education). Using the data points, we conducted an analysis to assess consistency of positive findings across risk factors and study designs (cohort vs. cross-sectional). In particular, we estimated the % of positive findings for all participants and most outcomes; % of positive findings for some outcomes or exposures but not others; % of positive findings for sub-groups; % null findings; % of findings against hypothesized direction. In addition, study-level characteristics (e.g. year and country of publication, study design, type of cognitive outcome, sample size, age group, sex) were described in detail and compared across risk factors, using χ2 test, independent samples t-test and one-way ANOVA.

Consistency analysis: all data points

In this part of the analysis, we modeled study finding as a binary outcome coded as 0=”null finding or finding against hypothesized direction” (referent category), 1=”positive or partially positive finding”, as a function of study-level characteristics using a logistic regression model. The study-level characteristics were entered as main effects as follows: (1) Year of publication; (2) Country of publication (1 = US, 2 = European country, 3 = Others), (3) risk factor (1 = education, 2 = smoking, 3 = alcohol, 4 = physical activity, 5 = caffeine, 6 = antioxidants, 7 = homocysteine, 8 = n-3 fatty acids); (4) sample size (when a range was provided, the average was taken), (5) Study participant age group: 1 = contains ages <65y, 0 = does not contain ages <65y; (6) Participant gender composition: 1 = Men only; 2 = Women only; 3 = Both; (7) Study design: 1 = cross-sectional; 2 = cohort; (8) Number of cognitive outcomes included in the study (e.g. 1 if only incident AD was the outcome; 2 if it is both incident AD and incident dementia); (9) General category of cognitive outcome(s): 1 = dementia/AD/impairment; 2 = cognitive function/decline; 3 = both.

Meta-analysis: data points with incident AD and selected risk or protective factors

Focusing on data points with incident AD as an outcome, we conducted further meta-analysis to assess the strength of the association between selected risk or protective factors and this outcome. This analysis was thus restricted to prospective cohort studies with available data points that had comparable measurements for each risk/protective factor, thus allowing to estimate a pooled measure of association across those data points and studies. The original reported odd ratios (ORs), relative risks (RRs) or Hazard Ratios (HRs) were combined into a pooled value with 95% confidence interval (CI). The RRs were then pooled using random effects models when included study data points were deemed heterogeneous based on the Q-test for homogeneity (p < 0.05) or fixed effect when study data points were homogenous (p > 0.05), which are also presented among results. As such, a summary or pooled RR was provided using forest plots and computed by computing the weighted average of the natural logarithm of each relative measure of interest weighting by the inverse of each RR’s respective variance [260]. Random effects models that further incorporated between-study variability were conducted using DerSimonian and Laird’s methodology.

Considering estimates of exposure prevalence from the largest study with available data on each exposure, we also computed a population attributable risk percentage (PAR%) by pooling data points from all studies together.

PAR%p,lcl,ucl;ij=100×Prexp×RRp,lcl,ucl;ij11+(PrexpRRp,lcl,ucl;ij1=1θij×100

Varθij=Var(1θij)=(1PARp;ij)2×Var(Ln(1PARp;ij))=1PARp;ij2×(Ln(1PARlcl;ij)Ln(1PARucl;ij)/3.92)2

PAR%95%CI;ij=PAR%p,ij±1.96×Varθij×100

As shown in Equations 1.1, 1.2 and 1.3, RR (point estimates per study and data point; 95% CI) was applied to the formula and Prexp was the estimated prevalence of each exposure. The estimation of SE for PAR% was obtained using the delta method [261].

Finally, in order to examine publication bias, we used Begg’s funnel plots; each RR point estimate was plotted against their corresponding standard errors (SE) for each study on a logarithmic scale [262,263], for all exposures combined. This type of bias was also formally tested using the Begg-adjusted rank correlation tests [264] and the Egger’s regression asymmetry test [265]. All analyses were conducted with STATA 11.0 (StataCorp, College Station, TX) [266]. Type I error was set at 0.05 for all measures of association.

Results and discussionSocio-economic Status (SES) as indicated by education

Early life conditions are related to cognitive development and abilities in childhood and cognitive function in adulthood. Low educational attainment and other markers of low socio-economic position (SEP) were associated with poorer cognitive function in adulthood and age-related cognitive decline and impairment, as well as greater risk or prevalence of dementia and AD in the elderly. In this study, we focused our attention on education as a maker of SES, given that it is the most commonly studied protective factor.

Several possible mechanisms support the finding that less education is related to cognitive decline: First, education may exert direct effects on brain structure early in life by increasing synapse number or vascularization and creating cognitive reserve. This was named the “reserve capacity” hypothesis. Thus, this hypothesis states that early life conditions affect the pace of cognitive decline in later life [38]. Education in early life may have effects in later life if persons with more education continue searching for mental stimulation (“the use it or lose it” hypothesis), which may lead to beneficial neurochemical or structural alterations in the brain [267]. Indeed, in one study, recent mental stimulation was associated with improved cognitive functioning [268]. Alternatively, education may act through several “behavioral mediators” to improve health in general, and cognitive functioning in particular [267]. This hypothesis was confirmed by a study using the Framingham cohort which suggested that education was uniquely protective against VaD and not associated with AD [28]. This finding was explained by mediating effects of other risk factors of cognitive decline, including smoking and hypertension, which in turn can initiate cerebrovascular damage. However, Lee and colleagues [52] found evidence contrary to this hypothesis by showing a sustained strong association between education and cognitive functioning after adjustment for behavioral and health-related factors.

Based on our findings (Table 2 and Figure 2A), 18 (66.7%) of the 27 cohort studies that met our selection criteria found that lower education was linked to a worse cognitive outcome in the overall population and for all studied outcomes, 1 found this relationship with incident VaD but not AD [28], 1 found the relationship with cognitive function but not decline [64], 1 concluded that IQ was a better predictor than education [38], and 2 detected a significant association in the hypothesized direction only in women [44] and in ApoE4- individuals [53]. The remaining four cohort studies did not find an association in the hypothesized direction [26,60,68,71].

Main findings (%) of selected studies, given hypothesis: (A) Cohort Studies (B) Cross-sectional studies.

Note: +(ALL) = positive finding, given hypothesis, for all subjects and most outcomes of interest; +(some outcomes) = positive finding, given hypothesis, for all subjects and some outcomes of interest but not others; +(some groups) = positive finding, given hypothesis, for some groups and most outcomes of interest; 0 = null finding, given hypothesis; − = finding against hypothesized direction. *P-value based on χ2 test for independence between risk factor and finding.

The association between education and the studied cognitive outcomes was in the hypothesized direction, with higher education being protective, for the majority of the selected cross-sectional studies (21 out of 25, 84%), while 2 found an association with prevalent AD but not VaD [41,56], 1 found that literacy was a better predictor than education [61], and 1 failed to detect a significant association [40] (See Table 2 and Figure 2B).

Behavioral factors

Several behavioral factors were selected, including smoking, alcohol drinking,and physical activity.

Smoking

Smoking is a risk factor for several chronic diseases, but its long-term relationship with dementia of various sub-types is still controversial. In fact, smoking is well known to increase the risk of stroke [269] and thus subsequent vascular types of dementia (VaD). However, many studies have concluded that smoking status influenced risk of VaD independently of stroke status and thus may have an effect beyond cerebrovascular disease. In addition, studies that have shown a direct impact of smoking on AD, suggest that smoking might in fact influence neurodegeneration. A vast amount of literature points to a role of smoking in oxidative stress and inflammation, both mechanisms believed to play a key role in AD [270].

However, it is also biologically plausible that smoking might protect against cognitive decline and AD, given that nicotine, a key active component of tobacco, may enhance the release of acetylcholine, increase the density of nicotnic receptors, therefore improving attention and information processing [271]. It is now known that AD is characterized by cholinergic system deficits which may be delayed through tobacco consumption [271,272].

Population-based evidence of an effect of smoking on cognitive outcomes was inconclusive, with most longitudinal studies reporting weak or null associations [63,72,74,76,77,82,84,90]. However, a number of other cohort studies have found a positive association between smoking and risk of incident dementia and AD [78,86,88,93,95,98,101,103,104] as well as incident cognitive impairment [75,81,90] and age-related cognitive decline [83,85,89,96,99,105].

For instance, the British 1946 birth cohort study pointed to the difficulty of finding an association between smoking and cognitive impairment given the differential high mortality of smokers especially among the elderly population [85]. After controlling for a range of socioeconomic and health status indicators (both physical and mental), they found that smokers who survive into later life may be at risk of clinically significant cognitive decline. However, these effects were accounted for largely by heavy smokers, i.e. those who smoked 20 cigarettes per day or more. Earlier research on middle aged adults suggested that current smoking and number of pack-years of smoking were related to reduced performance on tests of psychomotor speed and cognitive flexibility assessed approximately five years later [83]. Similar results were shown for cognitive decline in a large cohort study (Rotterdam study) conducted in multiple European countries [89] and in another more recent study conducted in the United States [92].

Among studies that examined incidence of AD in relation to smoking status, two of the largest European cohort studies reported conflicting results. While one found no relationship between smoking status and incident AD among a large sample of 34,439 older UK men (mean age: 81) [82], the recent 2011 study found that heavy smoking increased the risk of dementia and AD in a younger sample of 21,123 older Finnish adults (Mean age:60.1) that comprised both men and women [104].

In sum, 16 (55.2%) out of the 29 selected cohort studies linking smoking to the various cognitive outcomes found the relationship to be in the hypothesized direction in the entire population that was studied and for most outcomes of interest [75,81,83,85,86,88,89,92-96,98,99,101,104], while 2 found this relationship for some outcomes but not others [97,103] and 2 detected it for a sub-group of the total population [78,105], while the remaining 9 studies did not find an association [63,72,74,76,77,82,84,90] or found an association in the opposite direction [102]. (See Table 2 and Figure 2A).

Only 2 (28.6%) of the 7 cross-sectional studies found an association in the hypothesized direction [36,87], while 2 detected it for a sub-group of the total population [79,100], and the remaining 3 did not detect a significant association [73,80,91], (See Table 2 and Figure 2B).

Alcohol

Alcohol consumption in moderation was hypothesized to be protective against cognitive decline and impairment in old age. Several mechanisms may be involved in explaining the potential protective effect of moderate alcohol consumption on various cognitive outcomes. First, this effect might be mediated through cardiovascular risk factor reduction, partly through a dampening effect of ethanol on platelet aggregation, or through a modification of the serum lipid profile. Second, another potential mechanism in which alcohol can have a direct effect on cognitive function is through acetylcholine release in the hippocampus, which in turn enhances learning and memory [273].

The Rotterdam study [83] also examined the effect of alcohol use on cognition. They found that past alcohol consumption’s effect on speed and flexibility appeared to be slightly U-shaped, with the best performance observed among those who drank 1–4 glasses of alcohol per day, although this association was stronger among women than among men. Other studies also detected sex differences [106,107,111]. Light to moderate alcohol consumption was also found beneficial based on findings of other cohort and cross-sectional studies with a U- or J-shaped pattern observed [108-110,113,114,116,117,119-121,124-127]. However, in other studies, a linear dose–response relationship between alcohol use and improved cognition was noted, though the authors cautioned that these should not encourage increased alcohol consumption without an upper bound to this consumption [114,115,122,131].

In one cross-sectional study, a linear relationship between alcohol consumption and cognitive function was found in women but a U-shaped pattern was found in men [109]. One cohort study found that overall, moderate consumption was protective against poor cognitive function, but had an opposite relationship with cognitive function among ApoE4+ individuals [113], while another found that alcohol use in general was related to better cognition without effect modification by ApoE4 status [123]. Slower memory decline with increased alcohol consumption in men was found in one study, though the opposite relationship was found in the case of psychomotor speed among women [118]. The positive association between alcohol intake and memory was also noted in at least one other cross-sectional study for both men and women combined [128]. Moreover, heavy alcohol use was linked to poorer cognitive outcomes in a few studies [87,127,129,130]. Finally, only a few studies among those that were selected found no associations between alcohol consumption and cognitive outcomes [76,77,112].

In fact, 8 out of the 18 selected cohort studies (44%) linking alcohol consumption to the various cognitive outcomes, found the relationship to be in the hypothesized direction (but were U-shaped, J-shaped or linear) in the entire population that was studied and for most outcomes of interest [108,114,116,117,122,124,125,131], while 2 found this relationship for some outcomes but not others [115,120] and 4 detected it for a sub-group of the total population [83,111,113,118]. Moreover, 1 cohort studies have indicated that alcohol use was generally linked to poor cognitive outcomes for the total population [87]. Finally, 3 did not find any significant associations between alcohol consumption and the various cognitive outcomes that were under study [76,77,129]. (See Table 2 and Figure 2A).

9 of the 12 cross-sectional studies (75%) found an association in the hypothesized direction for the entire study population and for most outcomes of interest [106,109,110,119,121,123,126-128]. The remaining 3 studies either found this U-shaped or J-shaped association in a sub-group [107], and either failed to detect any association [112] or detected one that was not in line with the hypothesis, whereby alcohol use was generally found to result in poor cognitive outcomes [130]. (See Table 2 and Figure 2B).

Physical activity

Physical activity has many well-known benefits for preventing a number of chronic disorders, including coronary heart disease, stroke, diabetes mellitus and osteoporosis. However, its impact on cognitive functioning has not been studied extensively. Several mechanisms may underlie the potentially protective effects of physical activity on cognitive function, including sustained cerebral blood flow [274], improved aerobic capacity and cerebral nutrient supply [275,276] as well as growth factors, specifically the brain-derived neurotropic factor, which is a molecule that increases neuronal survival, enhances learning, and protects against cognitive decline [277,278].

Currently, 24 cohort and 4 cross-sectional studies have examined the hypothesized relationship. For instance, a recent cohort study of 716 dementia-free older adults from the Rush Memory and Aging Project who were followed-up for an average of 4 years found an inverse relationship between total daily physical activity and incident AD after controlling for age, sex, education, self-report physical, social, and cognitive activities, as well as current level of motor function, depressive symptoms, chronic health conditions, and ApoE4 allele status [156]. Furthermore, a recent cross-sectional study of 9344 women, 65 years and older, found a lower prevalence of cognitive impairment among those who reported being physically active versus those who reported being physically inactive at different stages of their lives [155].

These findings suggested that physical activity could represent an important and potent protective factor for cognitive decline and dementia in elderly persons. Significant findings were obtained by other recent cohort [132-145,147,148,150-154,156-158] and cross-sectional studies [100,146,149]. Only one cohort study resulted in non-significant findings [143].

In sum, of the 24 selected cohort studies linking physical activity to the various cognitive outcomes, 21(87.5%) found the relationship in the hypothesized direction in the entire population that was studied and for most outcomes of interest [132,133,135-140,142,144,145,147,148,150-154,156-158,279], while 1 found this relationship in ApoE4 carriers [134] and 1 in non-carriers [141]. In one cohort study, the association was against its hypothesized direction [143]. (See Table 2 and Figure 2A).

In addition, all 4 of the selected cross-sectional studies (100%) found an association in the hypothesized direction for the entire study population and for most outcomes of interest (See Table 2 and Figure 2B).

Nutritional factors

Nutritional factors being studied in relation to cognitive outcomes included caffeine consumpion, antioxidant nutrients and Hcy. In addition, special attention was devoted recently to one class of essential fatty acids, namely n-3 fatty acids.

Caffeine

Caffeine is known to be the most widely used psychoactive drug worldwide. Its main source is coffee particularly in Western diets. Acting as a stimulant of the central nervous system, caffeine causes heightened alertness and arousal [280]. Previous literature yielded inconsistent findings about the effects of caffeine consumption on cognitive processes. In fact, caffeine improved perceptual speed and vigilance, as well as more complex functions such as memory [281]. Caffeine is one type of compound known as methylxanthines whose effects are mainly to block adenosine receptors in the brain, resulting in cholinergic stimulation. It was hypothesized that such stimulation would lead to improved memory [282]. The earliest large cross-sectional study conducted by Jarvis and colleagues found that caffeine improved cognitive performance [159]. Later on, other cross-sectional studies focusing on tea consumption found similar results [100,162,167,170]. Others, however, did not show evidence of a significant protective effect [160,168]. In sum, 4 of 7 selected cross-sectional studies linking caffeine consumption to various cognitive outcomes found the relationship to be in the hypothesized direction in the study population and for most outcomes of interest (57.1%), one found this association in men [100] and two failed to find a significant association [160,168]. (See Table 2 and Figure 2B).

Of 11 cohort studies, positive findings pertained to 3 (27.3%) [163,164,175], though this was found only for coffee intake in two studies [165,169], while 5 recent studies detected this association only among women or for specific exposures [165,169,171-173]. The remaining cohort studies (3 of 11, 27%) did not find an association between caffeine intake and cognitive change [161] or incident dementia [166,174]. Given the paucity of large cohort studies, more research is needed to establish causality (See Table 2 and Figure 2A).

Antioxidants: focus on vitamin E

Several findings suggest that oxidative stress may play an important role in the pathogenesis of AD. First, the brains of AD patients have lesions that are associated with exposure to free radicals. Moreover, oxidative stress among these patients is also marked by an increased level of antioxidants in the brain that act as free radical scavengers. Finally, in vitro studies suggest that exogenous antioxidants may reduce the toxicity of β-amyloids in the brains of AD patients [283-285]. Based on these findings, it may be hypothesized that dietary antioxidants may help reduce the risk of AD.

Those epidemiologic studies examined the longitudinal relationship between supplemental antioxidants and risk of AD and other dementias found conflicting results: While vitamin C supplement use was related to lower AD risk in one cohort study [178], combined supplementation of vitamin E and vitamin C was associated with reduced prevalence and incidence of AD and cognitive decline in three other cohort studies [189,193,198], whereas another study found this effect to be specific to Vitamin E supplements [186]. These findings of a protective effect of supplemental antioxidant use against cognitive impairment and decline was replicated in a large cohort study [185]. However, there were only borderline or little evidence of a cognitive benefit from use of antioxidant supplements, particularly vitamins C and E, according to at least five independent cohort studies [177,180,187,192,199].

There are several prospective cohort studies on the effect of dietary antioxidants on the risk of dementia. One study found that high dietary intake of vitamins C and E may reduce the risk of AD [182] with the relationship most pronounced among smokers. Morris and colleagues [183] found that dietary intake of vitamin E, but not other antioxidants, was associated with a reduced risk of incident AD, although this association was restricted to individuals without the Apolipoprotein E ϵ4 genotype. Similar findings were reported with cognitive decline as an outcome [184]. In a later study when both outcomes were considered it was concluded that certain forms of tocopherols not found in dietary supplements but found only in foods may be at play [194]. This observation was corroborated by at least one recent study [197]. Another study, however, suggested that dietary antioxidants were not able to reduce AD risk [187]. Similarly, Laurin and colleagues [188] found no association between midlife dietary intake of vitamins E and C and dementia incidence. At least five other cohort studies came to a similar conclusion [176,181,201,202]. In addition to examining associations of cognition with vitamins C and E, other studies found that carotenoids, particularly β-carotene intake, may be have beneficial effects of various cognitive outcomes [176], though others were not able to detect such an association [184,201,202].

Irrespective of the source of antioxidants, plasma concentration may be a good biomarker for oxidative stress status. In particular, an inverse association between plasma vitamin E among others and poor cognitive outcomes was found in at least two cross-sectional studies [179,190] and two cohort studies [196,200]. Another cross-sectional study, however, did not find evidence of an association between plasma antioxidants, including vitamin E and prevalent AD [191]. In addition, among studies that examined the influence of plasma carotenoids [179,195], only one detected a significant potential protective effect against cognitive impairment [195]. While these results are mixed, they suggest that at least one antioxidant has a protective effect against adverse cognitive outcomes.

In sum, of the 21 selected cohort studies linking antioxidants, with focus on vitamin E, to the various cognitive outcomes, 9 (42.9%) found the relationship to be in the hypothesized direction in the entire population that was studied and for most outcomes of interest [176,178,182,184,189,193,194,197,198], while 5 found this relationship for specific antioxidants or some outcomes but not others [180,183,186,196,200,201] and 1 detected it for a sub-group of the total population [183]. The remaining selected cohort studies (n = 6) did not find a significant association [177,185,187,188,192,199]. (See Table 2 and Figure 2A).

Similarly, of the 6 cross-sectional studies that were selected, 2 (33.3%) found an association in the hypothesized direction for the entire study population and for most outcomes of interest [190,195], 1 found the association to hold only for vitamin E [179], whereas 3 found no significant association [181,191,202]. (See Table 2 and Figure 2B).

Homocysteine

An elevated level of plasma concentration of the sulfur amino acid Hcy (hyperhomocysteinemia) is recognized as an independent risk factor for cardiovascular, peripheral vascular, and cerebrovascular disease [286]. Accordingly, a potential influence of hyperhomocysteinemia on cognitive functioning among older adults was postulated and several studies were able to associate high levels of Hcy with increased risk of incident AD or all-cause dementia [206,217,224,228,233,234]. Studies have pointed to selective effect of Hcy on specific domains of cognition [214,287,288]. One explanation could be that Hcy might be affecting certain parts of the brain to a greater extent than others, and studies have linked Hcy to higher degree of white matter hyperintensities and with brain atrophy [289-293].

Even though blood Hcy levels increase with age and diminished renal function, it is largely determined by dietary intake of B-vitamins (mainly B-6 and B-12) and folate which are needed to convert Hcy into methionine and cysteine, through the methylation reactions [294]. Thus, Hcy status in plasma can be modified by dietary interventions. Moreover, vitamin B-12 plasma level has been shown to be inversely related to that of Hcy [295] and studies looking at Hcy levels and cognitive functioning also examined the effect of B-vitamins. In particular, vitamin B-12 was found to be protective against decline in at least three recent studies [204,213,219]. At least five other studies [204,213,214,216,217,219,221] concluded that fotate was protective against cognitive impairment or decline. For Vitamin B-6, two other studies suggested a protective effect [213,219]. An antagonistic interaction of folate and Vitamin B-12 with Hcy’s effect on cognition was noted in other studies [224,296,297]. Aside from its link to cardiovascular disease, Hcy was shown to have neurotoxic and excitotoxic properties in vitro[298,299], suggesting a direct influence on cognition.

Overall, of the 19 selected cohort studies linking Hcy to the various cognitive outcomes, 12(63.2%) found the relationship in the hypothesized direction in the entire population that was studied and for most outcomes of interest [206,215,217,219,222-224,228,231,233-235], while 2 found this relationship for some outcomes but not others or a sub-group [213,214] and 5 were not able to detect a significant association [203,209,210,229,230]. (See Table 2 and Figure 2A).

Similarly, of the 14 cross-sectional studies that were selected, 11(78%) found an association in the hypothesized direction for the entire study population and for most outcomes of interest [204,205,207,208,216,218,220,221,225,227,232], 1 found an association only among older adults above age 60y [212], 1 detected it among ApoE4+ individuals [226], and 1 found no significant relationship [211]. (See Table 2 and Figure 2B).

n-3 fatty acids

Another nutritional factor hypothesized to be protective against cognitive decline is higher intake of n-3 fatty acids and/or a better balance of n-3/n-6 fatty acids. Linoleic(LA ~ 18:2n-6) and α-linolenic (LNA ~ 18:3n-3) are two types of fatty acids that are essential for all members of the animal kingdom. These fatty acids and their respective derivatives are also commonly referred to as n-6 and n-3 fatty acids. Their essentiality lies in the fact that they cannot be synthesized de novo within the human or animal organism [300].

In the past, n-3 fatty acids were classified only as essential because of their ability to alleviate deficiency symptoms that include dermatitis, growth retardation and reproductive failure. However, n-3 fatty acids have other important neurological functions, which explain their high concentrations in neural and retinal tissues [301-303]. Some of the longer chain fatty acids that are synthesized from α-linolenic acid include Eicosapentanoic acid (EPA ~ 20:5 n-3), which through further elongation, desaturation and β-oxidation produces Docosahexaenoic acid (DHA ~ 22:6 n-3). On the other hand, products of linoleic acid which are also termed long-chain n-6 fatty acids include gamma-linoleic (GLA ~ 18:3 n-6), dihomogammalinolenic acid (DGLA ~ 20:3 n-6) and Arachidonic acid (AA ~ 20:4 n-6) [304]. Of all organs in the human body (excluding adipose tissue), the nervous system has the highest lipid content. The dry weight of an adult brain is 50% to 60% lipid, and 35% of the lipid content is accounted for by polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs) [305].

A review of scientific articles and biochemistry textbooks [306] suggested that the fatty acid composition of neuronal cell membrane phospholipids reflects their intake in the diet. Fish oils, which contain high levels of C20 and C22 PUFA, exert the most profound influence on brain PUFA concentrations [306]. The ratio between n-3 and n-6 PUFA may influence various aspects of serotoninergic and catecholaminergic neurotransmission, and it has been shown that by increasing the density of neurotransmitter receptors for acetylcholine and dopamine, dietary n-3 PUFA can improve learning and memory processes [307].

Previous observational studies suggested that the biochemical composition of blood components in terms of fatty acids differs significantly between subjects with normal cognitive functioning and patients with some form of cognitive impairment. While the majority of these studies showed an inverse association of plasma and erythrocyte n-3 fatty acids with cognition among older adults [243-245,248,255,258], at least one found no association between biochemical markers of n-3 fatty acids and cognition [251].

Epidemiological studies involving self-reported dietary data of n-3 fatty acids had suggestive but slightly controversial results. One study by Morris and colleagues used cohort data on 815 subjects who were initially unaffected by AD (age range: 65-94y, mean follow-up period = 2.3y). Using standardized criteria, AD incidence was compared across n-3 fatty acid consumption groups, with those eating fish once per week compared to those who rarely or never eat fish having considerably lower incidence (RR = 0.4; 95% CI: 0.2, 0.9). Total n-3 fatty acid consumption was also associated with a reduced AD risk even after controlling for intake of other dietary fats, vitamin E and for cardiovascular conditions [239]. A similar finding was reported later on for a larger but comparable cohort when looking at fish consumption and cognitive decline over time [241].

In the Zutphen Elderly Study, cognitive functioning and decline over three years were assessed in a cohort of 476 men aged 69-89y using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Findings indicated that high linoleic acid intake (the main n-6 fatty acid in the diet) was associated with cognitive impairment, even after controlling for age, education, cigarette smoking, alcohol consumption and energy intake (OR = 1.76, 95% CI: 1.04-3.01, comparing highest to lowest tertile). However, there was no distinctive association for n-3 fatty acids. Nevertheless, total fish consumption was suggestive of a protective effect, even though it did not reach significance [236].

Another larger cohort study–The Rotterdam Study– recruited 5,386 non-institutionalized participants, aged 55 + y at baseline, who had normal cognition and assessed their complete dietary intake with a semi-quantitative food-frequency questionnaire. After an average 2.1y of follow-up, lower risk of incident dementia and AD was found among fish consumers and therefore among those with higher intake of n-3 fatty acids (RR = 0.3; 95% CI: 0.1-0.9) [237]. However, when the study was conducted later with a longer follow-up (mean follow-up period of 6.0 years), it was concluded that high intake of total, saturated, trans fat, cholesterol and low intake of monounsatured fatty acids (MUFA), total PUFA, n-6 PUFA and n-3 PUFA were not associated with increased risk of dementia or its subtypes [238].

A cross-sectional study of 1,613 subjects aged 45–70 years that examined the association between fatty acid and fish intake with cognitive function, found that the risk of cognitive impairment was reduced with increased consumption of fatty fish and marine n-3 PUFA. per Standard Deviation (SD) increased intake, the ORs were 0.81 (95% CI: 0.66, 1.00) and 0.72 (95% CI: 0.57, 0.90), respectively [240]. Another recent study using the Athersclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) cohort data suggested that dietary intake of n-3 fatty acids (mainly DHA + EPA) reduced the risk of cognitive decline in verbal fluency but not other cognitive domains (i.e. delayed word recall and psychomotor speed). This protective effect was particularly strong among hypertensive subjects [246]. The potentially protective effect of dietary n-3 fatty acid was also reported in several other large epidemiological studies [242,247,249,253,254,256-258], but not in others [250-252].

In sum, 7 out of the 18 (39%) selected cohort studies linking n-3 fatty acids to the various cognitive outcomes found the relationship in the hypothesized direction in the entire population that was studied and for most outcomes of interest [237,239,247,248,250,253,257], while 1 found this relationship for some outcomes but not others [245], 4 detected it for a sub-group of the total population [242,243,246,258], and 6 found no association [236,238,241,250-252] (See Table 2 and Figure 2A). In addition, all of the 5 (100%) cross-sectional studies that were selected found an association in the hypothesized direction for the entire study population and for most outcomes of interest [240,244,249,254,255] (See Table 2 and Figure 2B).

Description of study-level characteristics and comparison by risk factor

Table 3 shows descriptive findings of study-level characteristics and compares their distributions across risk factors. Out of the 247 selected studies, 98 were conducted in the US (39.7%), while 104 were carried out in a European country (42.1%), and the remaining 45 studies originated from Asia, Canada and Australia among others (18.2%). The majority of the selected studies were cohort studies (n = 167). Most had only one type of cognitive outcome (72.5%), whereas 24.3% had two, and the remaining 3.2% had 3 or 4 outcomes. 152 studies had confirmed positive findings for most outcomes, exposures and for all study sub-groups (61.5%), while 18.2% (n = 45) had null findings. Partially positive findings were found in around 18.2% while 2% had a finding against the hypothesized direction. Around 40.5% of studies included participants with ages <65y, and the majority had both men and women (84.2%). Incident AD as an outcome was available in 47 studies, while 47 studies included incident dementia as a main outcome of interest. On the other hand, cognitive function as an outcome was found in 83 of included studies, while 62 of those studies had cognitive decline or change as a primary outcome of interest (data not shown). In general, there was an almost even split between studies focusing on cognitive function/decline/change (51.0%) and studies focused on AD/dementia/impairment as outcomes (46.2%). Only 2.8% of the studies examined both categories. When comparing the distribution of those study-level characteristics by risk factor, we found some significant differences for year of publication, country, age group inclusion/exclusion, study design, cognitive outcome type and study finding. In particular, studies on education and cognitive outcomes tended to be published earlier than studies of other risk factors, there were significantly more European studies of n-3 FA compared to other risk factors, while most studies with PA excluded middle aged adults unlike other risk factors. The highest proportion of cohort studies was also found for PA. The vast majority of studies on alcohol and cognitive outcomes used cognitive function/decline as their primary outcome of interest, unlike other risk factors which were more balanced in terms of cognitive outcome type. The percent positive finding was highest among PA studies (89.3%) and lowest for caffeine studies (38.9%). The significant difference in percent “positive finding” was found in cohort studies (p = 0.043) rather than cross-sectional studies (p = 0.09) (See Figure 2A-B).

Study-level characteristics distribution, overall and comparison across risk factors

 Overall
EDU
SMOK
ALCO
PA
CAFF
ANTIOX
HCY
N-FA
P *
N = 247N = 52N = 36N = 30N = 28N = 18N = 27N = 33N = 23
Year, Mean (SD)
2004.5
(5.1)
2001.3
(6.3)
2003.6
(5.2)
2004.5
(4.5)
2006.7
(3.6)
2007.7
(4.5)
2003.8
(3.8)
2006.2
(3.1)
2006.9
(4.0)
<0.001
Country, N (%)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
US
98
(39.7)
16
(30.8)
12
(33.3)
15
(50.0)
17
(63.0)
3
(16.7)
17
(63.0)
13
(39.4)
5
(21.7)
0.020
Europe
104
(42.1)
22
(42.3)
18
(50.0)
9
(30.0)
8
(28.6)
10
(55.6)
8
(29.6)
14
(42.4)
15
(65.2)
 
Other
45
(18.2)
14
(26.9)
6
(16.7)
6
(20.0)
3
(10.7)
5
(27.8)
2
(7.4)
6
(18.2)
3
(13.00
 
Age group, N (%)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Excludes < 65y
147
(59.5)
31
(59.6)
17
(47.2)
13
(43.3)
24
(85.7)
12
(66.7)
20
(74.1)
16
(48.5)
14
(60.9)
0.012
Includes < 65y
100
(40.5)
21
(40.4)
19
(52.8)
17
(56.7)
4
(14.3)
6
(33.3)
7
(25.9)
17
(51.5)
9
(39.1)
 
Sex, N (%)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Both
208
(84.2)
47
(90.4)
28
(77.8)
28
(93.4)
19
(67.9)
16
(88.9)
22
(81.5)
28
(84.8)
20
(87.0)
0.12
Men only
26
(10.5)
3
(5.8)
8
(22.2)
1
(3.3)
5
(17.9)
2
(11.1)
2
(7.4)
3
(9.1)
2
(8.7)
 
Women only
13
(5.3)
2
(3.8)
0
(0.0)
1
(3.3)
4
(14.3)
0
(0.0)
3
(11.1)
2
(6.1)
1
(4.3)
 
Study design, N (%)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Cross-sectional
80
(32.4)
25
(48.1)
7
(19.4)
12
(40.0)
4
(14.3)
7
(38.9)
6
(22.2)
14
(42.4)
5
(21.7)
0.012
Cohort
167
(67.6)
27
(51.9)
29
(80.6)
18
(60.0)
24
(85.7)
11
(61.1)
21
(77.8)
19
(57.6)
18
(78.3)
 
Sample size, Mean (SD)
3,561
(5,128)
4,345
(6,066)
4,745
(6,859)
4,745
(6,808)
3,322
(3,927)
2,408
(2,385)
3,643
(4,191)
1,074
(769)
3,061
(3,389)
0.05
Cognitive outcome count, N (%)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
179
(72.5)
34
(65.4)
26
(86.7)
26
(86.7)
20
(71.4)
14
(77.8)
22
(81.5)
21
(63.6)
15
(65.2)
0.77
2
60
(24.3)
15
(28.8)
4
(13.3)
4
(13.3)
7
(25.0)
3
(16.7)
4
(14.8)
10
(30.3)
8
(34.8)
 
3
5
(2.0)
2
(3.9)
0
(0.0)
0
(0.0)
1
(3.6)
1
(5.6)
0
(0.0)
1
(3.0)
0
(0.0)
 
4
2
(1.2)
1
(1.9)
0
(0.0)
0
(0.0)
0
(0.0)
0
(0.0)
1
(3.7)
1
(3.0)
0
(0.0)
 
Cognitive outcome type, N (%)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
0.042
AD/dementia/impairment
114
(46.2)
26
(50.0)
20
(55.6)
6
(20.0)
16
(57.1)
5
(27.8)
15
(55.6)
15
(45.5)
11
(47.8)
 
Cognitive function/decline
126
(51.0)
25
(48.1)
16
(44.4)
24
(80.0)
10
(35.7)
11
(61.1)
12
(44.4)
17
(51.5)
11
(47.8)
 
Both
7
(2.8)
1
(1.9)
0
(0.0)
0
(0.0)
2
(7.1)
2
(11.1)
0
(0.0)
1
(3.0)
1
(4.4)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Study finding, N(%)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
0.004
Against hypothesis
5
(2.0)
1
(1.9)
1
(2.8)
2
(6.7)
1
(3.6)
0
(0.0)
0
(0.0)
0
(0.0)
0
(0.0)
 
Null
45
(18.2)
4
(7.7)
11
(30.6)
4
(13.3)
0
(0.0)
5
(27.8)
9
(33.3)
6
(18.2)
6
(26.1)
 
Positive
152
(61.5)
39
(75.0)
18
(50.0)
17
(56.7)
25
(89.3)
7
(38.9)
11
(40.7)
23
(69.7)
12
(52.2)
 
Partially positive (outcomes/exposures)
21
(8.5)
6
(11.5)
2
(5.6)
2
(6.7)
0
(0.0)
3
(16.7)
2
(7.4)
1
(3.0)
1
(4.3)
 
Partially positive (sub-groups)24(9.7)2(7.7)4(11.1)5(16.7)2(7.1)3(16.7)5(27.8)3(9.1)4(17.4) 

*P-value for difference across risk/protective factors was obtained from one-way ANOVA test when variable is continuous and χ2 test when variable is categorical.

Consistency analysis: study-level characteristics and risk factor as predictors of study finding

In an attempt to examine heterogeneity in findings across risk factors and study-level characteristics, we conducted a consistency analysis using a logistic regression model (Table 4). Examining the odds ratios and their 95% CI, taking “null finding/against hypothesis finding” as the referent category for the outcome, we found that in general, a positive or partially positive finding was significantly more likely when the risk factor was “education” particularly when compared to smoking, caffeine and antioxidants/vitamin E (p < 0.05). None of the other study-level characteristics were associated with the study finding.

Multiple logistic regression: study-level predictors of study finding*

 Odds Ratio(95% CI)P-value
Year
1.06
(0.98;1.14)
0.13
Country
 
 
 
US
1.00
 
 
Europe
0.96
(0.45;2.06)
0.92
Other
0.99
(0.35;2.80)
0.99
Age group
 
 
 
Excludes < 65y
1.00
 
 
Includes < 65y
0.80
(0.40;1.60)
0.53
Study design
 
 
 
Cross-sectional
1.00
 
 
Cohort
0.60
(0.27;1.33)
0.21
Sample size
1.00
(0.99;1.00)
0.25
Cognitive outcome count
1.03
(0.54;1.98)
0.92
Cognitive outcome type
 
 
 
AD/dementia/impairment
1.00
 
 
Cognitive function/decline
1.52
(0.72;3.22)
0.27
Both
1.78
(0.16;19.7)
0.64
Risk factor
 
 
 
EDU
1.00
 
 
SMOK
0.21
(0.06;0.70)
0.012
ALCO
0.33
(0.08;1.29)
0.11
PA
2.49
(0.26;24.29)
0.43
CAFF
0.18
(0.04;0.84)
0.029
ANTIOX
0.20
(0.06;0.75)
0.016
HCY
0.41
(0.10;1.67)
0.22
N-3 FA0.25(0.06;1.05)0.06

*Positive or partially positive finding coded as “1”, Null or against hypothesized finding coded as “0” (referent category).

Gender composition of the sample was excluded as a predictor due to lack of variability.

Meta-analysis: selected risk factors for incident AD

Using random effects models, we pooled findings of 31 selected data points from 31 studies in which the outcome was incident AD and for which exposure data was adequate and comparable across studies (Figure 3A-E). Among studies used to summarize the association between low education and incident AD, the largest study (N = 3,675) found a HR = 1.81 with 95% CI: 1.30–2.53, with a prevalence of low education being ~32% [43]. In all four studies, the exposure definition was standardized as a comparison between <8y of education vs. ≥8y. In contrast, for all other exposures, definitions differed to some extent between studies but were assumed to operationalize the same concept. For instance, high vs. low physical activity was defined as a frequency of 3 times or more per week by two studies, 2 times of more per week by one, 4 activities per week by one, ≥2 vs. <2 mile walk/day by another study, and other comparable definitions by the remaining four studies combining frequency and intensity of activity. A full description of how various exposures were defined under Figure 3A-E notes.

Meta-analysis of selected risk and protective factors for incident AD (n = 31). (A) Education. (B) Smoking status. (C) Physical activity. (D) Homocysteine. (E) n3 fatty acids.

Sources:[28,33,43,46,78,82,86,88,93,95,101,103,104,133,137,141,142,145,148,150,156,206,209,215,217,233,237,239,242,250,251]: Notes: Only studies with available data points on incident AD were selected. Moreover, risk factors/protective factors needed to be measured in a comparable manner across studies to allow for estimating a pooled RR with a 95% CI. For education, only four studies out of 27 cohort studies had the required inclusion criteria. For smoking status, the common referent category was non-smoking or never smoking or « never or former » whereas exposed group consisted of either « ever smokers » [78,93], or a pooled value for RR to obtain an approximate « ever smoker » category [86], or current smokers [82,88,95], or mid-life smoking or heavy smoking [101,103,104]. For high vs. low physical activity level, two studies used the cut-point of 3 or more times per week [133,145], one used 2 times or more per week [142], one used the criterion of 4 activities per week vs. none [141], one used > =2 vs. <2 miles walking/day [137], and the remaining four studies used other definitions related to both frequency and intensity [148,150,156]. For high vs. low n-3 fatty acids, one study had fish consumption (yes vs. no) as the exposure of interest [237], another examined quintiles of total n-3 PUFA and compared the fifth to the first quintile in terms of risk for AD [239], a third study had one main exposure as « weekly consumption of fish vs. not » [242], a fourth study contrast high vs. no fish intake, 0-8y follow-up [250], and finally upper quartile vs. lowest quartile for total n-3 PUFA in erythrocyte membranes [251]. For high vs. low Hcy, two studies used a cut-point of 14.6 μmol/L [206,215], one study used a cut-point of 15 μmol/L [217], one study used upper vs. lowest quartile [209] and one study used upper tertile vs. lowest tertile [233].

Tests of heterogeneity, including the Q-tests, determined whether to use fixed-effects or random-effects models to pool the RR. Findings indicated that, with the exception of education and smoking as main exposures, RR estimates obtained from individual studies were largely homogenous. In sum, the pooled RRs were: 1.99 (1.30, 3.04) for low vs. higher educational attainment (n = 4 data points; Q = 11.33, p = 0.010); 1.37(1.23-1.52) for smoking status (current or ever vs. never smokers (n = 9 data points, Q = 36.2, p < 0.001); 0.58(0.49, 0.70) for physical activity (n = 8 data points, Q = 3.2, p = 0.867), 0.67(0.47,0.96) for high intake of n-3 fatty acids (n = 5, Q = 7.4, p = 0.116), and RR = 1.93(1.50, 2.49) for high levels of plasma Hcy (n = 5 data points; Q = 2.64, p = 0.620).

Taking the largest study for each as a means to obtain an estimate of exposure prevalence, the following was found: low education (32%) [43], mid-life smoking (59.8%) [93], lower physical activity (62%) [150], lower n-3 fatty acids (49.4%) [242], elevated Hcy (30%) [206]. From these exposure prevalence estimates (Prevexp), we computed the PAR% and its 95% CI to assess the proportion of AD that is attributable to each exposure in a typical adult population and thus the % that can be averted if that exposure was eliminated from that population. Our findings indicated that the PAR% for low education was 24.0% with a 95% CI: 8.4-39.6; for mid-life smoking it was 31.0% with a 95% CI: 17.9-44.3; for physical activity (lower vs. higher) it was 31.9% with 95% CI: 22.7-41.2; for high vs. low Hcy, it was 21.7% with a 95% CI: 12.8-30.6; for lower vs. higher fish consumption (<weekly vs. ≥weekly), it was 21.9% with 95% CI:4.7-39.1.

Publication bias for the meta-analysis data points (n = 31) was assessed using primarily the funnel plot which plotted point estimates of RR for all exposures combined on the Loge scale against their standard errors. This plot indicated that estimates obtained from those 31 studies lay within the pseudo 95% confidence limits, an indication of non-appreciable publication bias. This finding was reinforced by a non-significant Begg-adjusted rank correlation test (Z = 0.25; P = 0.80), and by Egger’s regression asymmetry test (bias (SE): -0.43 (0.98); p = 0.66) (Additional file 1: Figure S1).

Discussion

As stated earlier, this is the first study to systematically review those selected modifiable risk and protective factors for cognitive health outcomes in cross-sectional and cohort studies while comparing the consistency of association between those factors and across study-level characteristics. It is also among few recent studies to compare the strength of association across those factors in relation to incident AD using a similar approach [19,20]. However, our study has a few limitations. First, the literature search was limited to published articles in English available in the Medline database. Second, comparing all included studies in a quantitative meta-analytic manner was not possible due to the diversity of the cognitive outcome measurements between original studies included. In fact, cognitive measures included scores from batteries of different cognitive tests, single global cognitive test scores such as the MMSE total score, as well as the use of a factor analytic approach to combine test scores into various domains of interest (e.g. memory, spatial, psychomotor, executive function, attention). Thus, meta-analysis was only possible for measures of cognitive impairment (i.e. MCI, all-cause dementia, AD, VaD) from which we selected incident AD as the most comparable outcome across studies. However, in order to measure consistency across the selected studies and compare risk factors in terms of consistency, we conducted another type of analysis in which a qualitative outcome of “study finding” was modeled against study-level characteristics. The qualification of a finding as null or positive was based on the main conclusion of each study that was included. This type of analysis does not necessarily discriminate between null findings due to low power, poor quality study vs. actual null finding. However, our logistic regression analysis indicated that overall, sample size was not a determining factor for the study finding outcome. Combining findings from meta-analysis and the consistency analyses, we compared evidence level for each risk and protective factor of cognitive health. Other limitations include the lack of comparability in measurements of risk or protective factors in studies with incident AD, which resulted in the exclusion of a few data points in our meta-analysis. However, the datapoints that were included in the meta-analysis were relatively comparable as described in the footnotes of Figure 3A-E. Finally, our study was limited by the inability to create a common quality measure for all studies given the diversity of the exposure variables and the relative importance of having a large sample size given the type of exposure (e.g., a larger sample size is needed for a questionnaire-based exposure vs. a blood level based exposure).

Our review shows that over the past several decades many risk or protective factors have been studied in relation to cognitive impairment, dementia (including AD) and cognitive decline. Overall, these studies indicate that modifiable factors including individuals’ socio-economic, behavioral characteristics and dietary intake seem to affect people’s cognitive ability and change over time, as well as the incidence of cognitive impairment, all-cause dementia and AD. It is worth noting, however, that some of the diagnostic criteria for dementia, AD and MCI have changed over time, particularly between the 1990s and the more recent years, as shown in Table 1.

In total, 247 studies were retrieved for systematic review. When conducting consistency analysis for each risk factor/design dyad, we found the % of studies with positive finding, given hypothesis, for most outcomes and study participants to range from ~38.9% for caffeine (27.3 for cohort studies (n = 11), 57.1% for cross-sectional studies(n = 7)) to ~89% for physical activity(87.5% for cohort studies(n = 24); 100% for cross-sectional studies(n = 4)). Consistency analysis confirmed that education-related studies had a significantly higher propensity for a positive or partially positive finding compared to caffeine, smoking and antioxidant-related studies. Meta-analysis of 31 studies with incident AD and selected risk/protective factors yielded pooled RR and 95% CI as follows: RR = 1.99(1.30, 3.04) for low(risk factor) vs. higher education (n = 4 studies; Q = 11.33, p = 0.010); RR = 1.37(1.23, 1.52) for smoking status (current or ever(risk factor) vs. never smokers (n = 9 studies, Q = 36.2, p < 0.001); RR = 0.58(0.49, 0.70) for higher physical activity(protective factor) vs. lower (n = 8 studies, Q = 3.2, p = 0.867), RR = 0.67(0.47,0.96) for higher intake of n-3 fatty acids(protective factor) vs. lower (n = 5, Q = 7.4, p = 0.116), and RR = 1.93(1.50, 2.49) for high levels of plasma Hcy(risk factor) vs. lower (n = 6 data points; Q = 2.64, p = 0.620). Given the observed prevalence of exposure from the largest study per risk factor included in each meta-analysis, the population attributable risk percent (PAR%) with its 95% CI was estimated as follows: for low education: 24.0% with a 95% CI: 8.4-39.6; for mid-life smoking it was 31.0% with a 95% CI: 17.9-44.3; for physical activity (lower vs. higher) it was 31.9% with 95% CI: 22.7-41.2; for high vs. low Hcy, it was 21.7% with a 95% CI: 12.8-30.6; for lower vs. higher fish consumption (<weekly vs. ≥weekly), it was 21.9% with 95% CI:4.7-39.1. There was no significant publication bias, taking all selected risk factors for incident AD together.

A large number of epidemiologic studies were initially conducted to examine the effects of socio-economic factors, mainly educational attainment, and were later used to assess the validity of alternative hypotheses regarding the presence of behavioral or health-related mediating factors. To this end, several behavioral and nutritional risk factors were studied in relation to various cognitive outcomes. For instance, it was hypothesized that low SES was associated with higher prevalence of smoking which in turn may affect cognitive performance and change over time. While few studies found weak or no association between smoking and cognitive decline, many others found a positive association whereby smoking increased the risk of decline. Alcohol was found in general to have a U-shaped association with the risk of decline, while caffeine was shown to increase perceptual speed and vigilance as well as memory and other more complex functions in at least two cohort studies and one cross-sectional study. Physical activity was shown to protect against cognitive decline as corroborated by a number of prospective cohort studies.

Among nutritional factors, dietary and supplemental antioxidants were shown in some studies to reduce the risk of cognitive decline while in others they showed no appreciable effect. Other micronutrients including B-Vitamins and folate were shown to be protective against cognitive decline, through their dampening effect on plasma Hcy which was shown to consistently increase the risk of dementia, particularly of the AD type. In addition, studies show that n-3 fatty acids with their anti-inflammatory and cardio-protective properties can help reduce the risk of cognitive decline and impairment in some studies but not in others. Among nutritional factors, caffeine seems to be the factor hypothesized to have a protective effect with the smallest number of current studies.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the consistency of findings between studies varied for each selected risk or protective modifiable factors (highest consistency observed for physical activity). Secondly, a moderate to strong association was observed between some selected factors and incident AD (strongest for low education and elevated Hcy). Combining both criteria (strength of association in the case of incident AD and consistency overall), the strongest evidence thus far is an increased risk with elevated plasma Hcy levels or lower educational attainment and a lowered risk with increased physical activity. Nevertheless, more studies are needed to verify the consistency, particularly regarding caffeine. A comprehensive meta-analysis requires additional research for certain risk factors of incident AD or dementia. For incident AD, selected risk factors may potentially account on average for 21.7%-31.9% of AD cases for each risk factor considered (with 95% CI: 8.4%-44.3%) (highest for mid-life smoking and physical activity), given the estimated prevalence of those factors from the largest available study. Thus, on average, one in five to one in three cases of AD can potentially be averted if those risk factors were eliminated from populations with comparable exposure prevalence.

Abbreviations

AD: Alzheimer’s disease; VaD: Vascular dementia; DLB: Dementia with Lewy bodies; PD-D: Parkinson’s disease with dementia; MD: Mixed dementia; FTD: Fronot-parietal dementia; OD: Other dementia; MCI: Mild cognitive impairment; ADDTC: Alzheimer's disease diagnostic and treatment centers; DSM-IV: Diagnostic and statistical manual, 4th edition; ICD-10: International classification of disease, 10th edition; NINCDS-ADRDA: National Institute of neurological and communicative disorders and stroke -- the Alzheimer's disease and related disorders association; NINDS-AIREN: National institute of neurological and communicative disorders and stroke--Association Internationale pour la Recherche et l’Enseignement en Neurosciences; ApoE: Apolipoprotein E; PAR%: Population attributable risk percent; OR: Odd ratio; RR: Relative risk; HR: Hazard ratio; CI: Confidence interval; Prexp: Prevalence of each exposure; SE: Standard errors; SD: Standard deviation; Edu: Education; Smok: Smoking status; Alco: Alcohol consumption; PA: Physical activity; CAFF: Caffeine consumption; Antiox: Antioxidants; N-3 Fa: n-3 fatty acids; SES: Socio-economic status; SEP: Socio-economic position; Hcy: Homocysteine; EPA: Eicosapentanoic acid; DHA: Docosahexaenoic acid; DGLA: Dihomogaminalinolenic acid; GLA: Gamma-linoleic acid; AA: Arachidonic acid; PUFA: Polyunsaturated fatty acid; MUFA: monounsaturated fatty acid; MMSE: Mini-mental state examination; B: Both; M: Men; W: Women; UK: United Kingdom; US: United States.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MAB: conceptualization, literature search and review, plan of analysis, data management, statistical analysis (including meta-analysis), writing of the manuscript, revision of the manuscript. HAB: Literature search and review, plan of analysis, writing of parts of the manuscript, revision of the manuscript. AG: Literature search and review, plan of analysis, writing of parts of the manuscript, revision of the manuscript. AT: Literature search and reivew, writing of parts of the manuscript, revision of the manuscript. ABZ: Plan of analysis, write-up of parts of the manuscript, revision of the manuscript. YW: Plan of analysis, write-up of parts of the manuscript, revision of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

Youfa Wang and Alan B Zonderman are senior authors.

Pre-publication history

The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed here:

http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2458/14/643/prepub

Supplementary MaterialAdditional file 1: Figure S1

Begg’s funnel plot with pseudo 95% confidence limits.

Click here for file

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Aging, NIA/NIH/IRP. The authors would like to thank Mr. James Byrnes, NIA/NIH/IRP, for help with literature search and retrieval. Special thanks also go to Drs. Lori L. Beason-Held and Madhav Thambisetty, NIA/NIH/IRP, for their review and comments on the manuscript.

de ChamplainJWuRGirouardHKarasMELMALaplanteMAWuLOxidative stress in hypertensionClin Exp Hypertens20042659360115702613VerhaeghenPSalthouseTAMeta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural modelsPsychol Bull19971222312499354147MoritzDJKaslSVBerkmanLFCognitive functioning and the incidence of limitations in activities of daily living in an elderly community sampleAm J Epidemiol199514141497801965LoboALaunerLJFratiglioniLAndersenKDi CarloABretelerMMCopelandJRDartiguesJFJaggerCMartinez-LageJSoininenHHofmanAPrevalence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: a collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurologic Diseases in the Elderly Research GroupNeurology200054S4910854354LiGShenYCChenCHZhaoYWLiSRLuMAn epidemiological survey of age-related dementia in an urban area of BeijingActa Psychiatr Scand1989795575632788352FichterMMMellerISchroppelHSteinkirchnerRDementia and cognitive impairment in the oldest old in the community. Prevalence and comorbidityBr J Psychiatry19951666216297620747AnkriJPoupardMPrevalence and incidence of dementia among the very old. Review of the literatureRev Epidemiol Sante Publique20035134936013130215JormAFJolleyDThe incidence of dementia: a meta-analysisNeurology1998517287339748017OttABretelerMMvan HarskampFClausJJvan der CammenTJGrobbeeDEHofmanAPrevalence of Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia: association with education. The Rotterdam studyBMJ19953109709737728032LiuHCLinKNTengELWangSJFuhJLGuoNWChouPHuHHChiangBNPrevalence and subtypes of dementia in Taiwan: a community survey of 5297 individualsJ Am Geriatr Soc1995431441497836638McKhannGDrachmanDFolsteinMKatzmanRPriceDStadlanEMClinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of health and human services task force on Alzheimer's diseaseNeurology1984349399446610841McKhannGMKnopmanDSChertkowHHymanBTJackCRJrKawasCHKlunkWEKoroshetzWJManlyJJMayeuxRMohsRCMorrisJCRossorMNScheltensPCarrilloMCThiesBWeintraubSPhelpsCHThe diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's diseaseAlzheimers Dement2011726326921514250RomanGCTatemichiTKErkinjunttiTCummingsJLMasdeuJCGarciaJHAmaducciLOrgogozoJMBrunAHofmanAVascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN international workshopNeurology1993432502608094895HachinskiVCIliffLDZilhkaEDu BoulayGHMcAllisterVLMarshallJRussellRWSymonLCerebral blood flow in dementiaArch Neurol1975326326371164215The Lund and Manchester GroupsClinical and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia. The Lund and Manchester groupsJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry1994574164188163988McKeithIGGalaskoDKosakaKPerryEKDicksonDWHansenLASalmonDPLoweJMirraSSByrneEJLennoxGQuinnNPEdwardsonJAIncePGBergeronCBurnsAMillerBLLovestoneSCollertonDJansenENBallardCde VosRAWilcockGKJellingerKAPerryRHConsensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the consortium on DLB international workshopNeurology199647111311248909416CreceliusCDiagnosis and treatment of non-Alzheimer's dementiasJ Am Med Dir Assoc20034H252912857366ZekryDHauwJJGoldGMixed dementia: epidemiology, diagnosis, and treatmentJ Am Geriatr Soc2002501431143812165002GorelickPBScuteriABlackSEDecarliCGreenbergSMIadecolaCLaunerLJLaurentSLopezOLNyenhuisDPetersenRCSchneiderJATzourioCArnettDKBennettDAChuiHCHigashidaRTLindquistRNilssonPMRomanGCSellkeFWSeshadriSAmerican Heart Association Stroke Council, Council on Epidemiology and Prevention, Council on Cardiovascular Nursing, Council on Cardiovascular Radiology and Intervention, and Council on Cardiovascular Surgery and AnesthesiaVascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia: a statement for healthcare professionals from the american heart association/american stroke associationStroke2011422672271321778438BarnesDEYaffeKThe projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer's disease prevalenceLancet Neurol20111081982821775213ZhangMYKatzmanRSalmonDJinHCaiGJWangZYQuGYGrantIYuELevyPThe prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease in Shanghai, China: impact of age, gender, and educationAnn Neurol1990274284372353798OgunniyiALekwauwaUGOsuntokunBOInfluence of education on aspects of cognitive functions in non-demented elderly NigeriansNeuroepidemiology1991102462501798426DealbertoMJGagnonMBarberger-GateauPDartiguesJFAlperovitchA[Influence of educational status on a screening test for dementia, the mini-mental state examination]Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique199240931011631382KoivistoKHelkalaELReinikainenKJHanninenTMykkanenLLaaksoMPyoralaKRiekkinenPJPopulation-based dementia screening program in Kuopio: the effect of education, age, and sex on brief neuropsychological testsJ Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol199251621711497793EvansDABeckettLAAlbertMSHebertLEScherrPAFunkensteinHHTaylorJOLevel of education and change in cognitive function in a community population of older personsAnn Epidemiol1993371778287159PaykelESBrayneCHuppertFAGillCBarkleyCGehlhaarEBeardsallLGirlingDMPollittPO'ConnorDIncidence of dementia in a population older than 75 years in the United KingdomArch Gen Psychiatry1994513253328161293WhiteLKatzmanRLosonczyKSaliveMWallaceRBerkmanLTaylorJFillenbaumGHavlikRAssociation of education with incidence of cognitive impairment in three established populations for epidemiologic studies of the elderlyJ Clin Epidemiol1994473633747730861CobbJLWolfPAAuRWhiteRD'AgostinoRBThe effect of education on the incidence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease in the Framingham StudyNeurology199545170717127675231FarmerMEKittnerSJRaeDSBartkoJJRegierDAEducation and change in cognitive function. The epidemiologic catchment area studyAnn Epidemiol19955177728280CallahanCMHallKSHuiSLMusickBSUnverzagtFWHendrieHCRelationship of age, education, and occupation with dementia among a community-based sample of African AmericansArch Neurol1996531341408639062HanninenTKoivistoKReinikainenKJHelkalaELSoininenHMykkanenLLaaksoMRiekkinenPJPrevalence of ageing-associated cognitive decline in an elderly populationAge Ageing1996252012058670552ChristensenHKortenAEJormAFHendersonASJacombPARodgersBMackinnonAJEducation and decline in cognitive performance: compensatory but not protectiveInt J Geriatr Psychiatry1997123233309152716EvansDAHebertLEBeckettLAScherrPAAlbertMSChownMJPilgrimDMTaylorJOEducation and other measures of socioeconomic status and risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a defined population of older personsArch Neurol199754139914059362989FreidlWSchmidtRStroneggerWJReinhartBThe impact of sociodemographic, environmental, and behavioral factors, and cerebrovascular risk factors as potential predictors of the mattis dementia rating scaleJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci199752M1111169060979KalmijnSFeskensEJLaunerLJKromhoutDLongitudinal study of the effect of apolipoprotein e4 allele on the association between education and cognitive decline in elderly menBMJ199731434359001477KilanderLNymanHBobergMLithellHCognitive function, vascular risk factors and education. A cross-sectional study based on a cohort of 70-year-old menJ Intern Med19972423133219366810SchmandBSmitJLindeboomJSmitsCHooijerCJonkerCDeelmanBLow education is a genuine risk factor for accelerated memory decline and dementiaJ Clin Epidemiol199750102510339363037SchmandBSmitJHGeerlingsMILindeboomJThe effects of intelligence and education on the development of dementia. A test of the brain reserve hypothesisPsychol Med199727133713449403905CerhanJRFolsomARMortimerJAShaharEKnopmanDSMcGovernPGHaysMACrumLDHeissG Correlates of cognitive function in middle-aged adults. Atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) study investigatorsGerontology199844951059523221De RonchiDFratiglioniLRucciPPaternicoAGrazianiSDalmonteEThe effect of education on dementia occurrence in an Italian population with middle to high socioeconomic statusNeurology199850123112389595968LinRTLaiCLTaiCTLiuCKYenYYHowngSLPrevalence and subtypes of dementia in southern Taiwan: impact of age, sex, education, and urbanizationJ Neurol Sci199816067759804120LyketsosCGChenLSAnthonyJCCognitive decline in adulthood: an 11.5-year follow-up of the Baltimore epidemiologic catchment area studyAm J Psychiatry199915658659892298LetenneurLGilleronVCommengesDHelmerCOrgogozoJMDartiguesJFAre sex and educational level independent predictors of dementia and Alzheimer's disease? Incidence data from the PAQUID projectJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry19996617718310071096OttAvan RossumCTvan HarskampFvan de MheenHHofmanABretelerMMEducation and the incidence of dementia in a large population-based study: the Rotterdam studyNeurology19995266366610025813AevarssonOSkoogIA longitudinal population study of the mini-mental state examination in the very old: relation to dementia and educationDement Geriatr Cogn Disord20001116617510765048QiuCBackmanLWinbladBAguero-TorresHFratiglioniLThe influence of education on clinically diagnosed dementia incidence and mortality data from the Kungsholmen projectArch Neurol2001582034203911735777AlvaradoBEZunzuneguiMVDel SerTBelandFCognitive decline is related to education and occupation in a Spanish elderly cohortAging Clin Exp Res20021413214212092786CagneyKALauderdaleDSEducation, wealth, and cognitive function in later lifeJ Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci200257P16317211867664HerreraEJrCaramelliPSilveiraASNitriniREpidemiologic survey of dementia in a community-dwelling Brazilian populationAlzheimer Dis Assoc Disord20021610310812040305RavagliaGFortiPMaioliFSacchettiLMarianiENativioVTalericoTVettoriCMaciniPLEducation, occupation, and prevalence of dementia: findings from the Conselice studyDement Geriatr Cogn Disord2002149010012145456WightRGAneshenselCSSeemanTEEducational attainment, continued learning experience, and cognitive function among older menJ Aging Health20021421123611995741LeeSKawachiIBerkmanLFGrodsteinFEducation, other socioeconomic indicators, and cognitive functionAm J Epidemiol200315771272012697575SeemanTEHuangMHBretskyPCrimminsELaunerLGuralnikJMEducation and APOE-e4 in longitudinal cognitive decline: MacArthur studies of successful agingJ Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci200560P748315746021LeeSBuringJECookNRGrodsteinFThe relation of education and income to cognitive function among professional womenNeuroepidemiology2006269310116352912ShadlenMFSiscovickDFitzpatrickALDulbergCKullerLHJacksonSEducation, cognitive test scores, and black-white differences in dementia riskJ Am Geriatr Soc20065489890516776783ZhangZXZahnerGERománGCLiuXHWuCBHongZHongXTangMNZhouBQuQMZhangXJLiHSocio-demographic variation of dementia subtypes in china: methodology and results of a prevalence study in Beijing, Chengdu, Shanghai, and XianNeuroepidemiology20062717718717035714ZhouDFWuCSQiHFanJHSunXDComoPQiaoYLZhangLKieburtzKPrevalence of dementia in rural China: impact of age, gender and educationActa Neurol Scand200611427328016942548GalaskoDSalmonDGamstAOlichneyJThalLJSilbertLKayeJBrooksPAdonayRCraigUKSchellenbergGBorensteinARPrevalence of dementia in Chamorros on Guam: relationship to age, gender, education, and APOENeurology2007681772178117515539van HoorenSAValentijnAMBosmaHPondsRWvan BoxtelMPJollesJCognitive functioning in healthy older adults aged 64–81: a cohort study into the effects of age, sex, and educationNeuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn200714405417164189ChristensenHBatterhamPJMackinnonAJAnsteyKJWenWSachdevPSEducation, atrophy, and cognitive change in an epidemiological sample in early old ageAm J Geriatr Psychiatry20091721822619454848DotsonVMKitner-TrioloMHEvansMKZondermanABEffects of race and socioeconomic status on the relative influence of education and literacy on cognitive functioningJ Int Neuropsychol Soc20091558058919573276GavrilaDAntunezCTormoMJCarlesRGarcia SantosJMParrillaGFortunaLJimenezJSalmeronDNavarroCPrevalence of dementia and cognitive impairment in Southeastern Spain: the Ariadna studyActa Neurol Scand200912030030719832772PetersRBeckettNGenevaMTzekovaMLuFHPoulterRGainsboroughNWilliamsBde VernejoulMCFletcherABulpittCSociodemographic and lifestyle risk factors for incident dementia and cognitive decline in the HYVETAge Ageing20093852152719553357WilsonRSHebertLEScherrPABarnesLLde Leon CFMEvansDAEducational attainment and cognitive decline in old ageNeurology20097246046519188578HamidTAKrishnaswamySAbdullahSSMomtazYASociodemographic risk factors and correlates of dementia in older MalaysiansDement Geriatr Cogn Disord20103053353921252548MathuranathPSCherianPJMathewRKumarSGeorgeAAlexanderARanjithNSarmaPSDementia in Kerala, South India: prevalence and influence of age, education and genderInt J Geriatr Psychiatry20102529029719621355ScazufcaMAlmeidaOPMenezesPRThe role of literacy, occupation and income in dementia prevention: the Sao Paulo ageing & health study (SPAH)Int Psychogeriatr2010221209121520678301Castro-CostaEDeweyMEUchoaEFirmoJOLima-CostaMFStewartRTrajectories of cognitive decline over 10 years in a Brazilian elderly population: the Bambui cohort study of agingCad Saude Publica201127Suppl 3S34535021952855MarengoniAFratiglioniLBandinelliSFerrucciLSocioeconomic status during lifetime and cognitive impairment no-dementia in late life: the population-based aging in the Chianti area (InCHIANTI) studyJ Alzheimers Dis20112455956821297261Mejia-ArangoSGutierrezLMPrevalence and incidence rates of dementia and cognitive impairment no dementia in the Mexican population: data from the Mexican health and aging studyJ Aging Health2011231050107421948770ZahodneLBGlymourMMSparksCBontempoDDixonRAMacDonaldSWManlyJJEducation does not slow cognitive decline with aging: 12-year evidence from the victoria longitudinal studyJ Int Neuropsychol Soc2011171039104621923980HerbertLEScherrPABeckettLAAlbertMSRosnerBTaylorJOEvansDARelation of smoking and low-to-moderate alcohol consumption to change in cognitive function: a longitudinal study in a defined community of older personsAm J Epidemiol19931378818918484379LetenneurLDartiguesJFCommengesDBarberger-GateauPTessierJFOrgogozoJMTobacco consumption and cognitive impairment in elderly people. A population-based studyAnn Epidemiol199444494547804499FordABMefroucheZFriedlandRPDebanneSMSmoking and cognitive impairment: a population-based studyJ Am Geriatr Soc1996449059098708298GalanisDJPetrovitchHLaunerLJHarrisTBFoleyDJWhiteLR Smoking history in middle age and subsequent cognitive performance in elderly Japanese-American men. The Honolulu-Asia Aging StudyAm J Epidemiol19971455075159063340BroeGACreaseyHJormAFBennettHPCaseyBWaiteLMGraysonDACullenJHealth habits and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in old age: a prospective study on the effects of exercise, smoking and alcohol consumptionAust N Z J Public Health1998226216239744220EdelsteinSLKritz-SilversteinDBarrett-ConnorEProspective association of smoking and alcohol use with cognitive function in an elderly cohortJ Womens Health19987127112819929860OttASlooterAJHofmanAvan HarskampFWittemanJCVan BroeckhovenCvan DuijnCMBretelerMMSmoking and risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease in a population-based cohort study: the Rotterdam studyLancet1998351184018439652667CarmelliDSwanGEReedTSchellenbergGDChristianJCThe effect of apolipoprotein E epsilon4 in the relationships of smoking and drinking to cognitive functionNeuroepidemiology19991812513310202267ElwoodPCGallacherJEHopkinsonCAPickeringJRabbittPStolleryBBrayneCHuppertFABayerASmoking, drinking, and other life style factors and cognitive function in men in the Caerphilly cohortJ Epidemiol Community Health19995391410326046CervillaJAPrinceMMannASmoking, drinking, and incident cognitive impairment: a cohort community based study included in the Gospel Oak projectJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry20006862262610766894DollRPetoRBorehamJSutherlandISmoking and dementia in male British doctors: prospective studyBMJ20003201097110210775216KalmijnSvan BoxtelMPVerschurenMWJollesJLaunerLJCigarette smoking and alcohol consumption in relation to cognitive performance in middle ageAm J Epidemiol200215693694412419766ChenWTWangPNWangSJFuhJLLinKNLiuHCSmoking and cognitive performance in the community elderly: a longitudinal studyJ Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol200316182212641368RichardsMJarvisMJThompsonNWadsworthMECigarette smoking and cognitive decline in midlife: evidence from a prospective birth cohort studyAm J Public Health20039399499812773367TyasSLWhiteLRPetrovitchHWebster RossGFoleyDJHeimovitzHKLaunerLJMid-life smoking and late-life dementia: the Honolulu-Asia aging studyNeurobiol Aging20032458959612714116ZhouHDengJLiJWangYZhangMHeHStudy of the relationship between cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking and cognitive impairment among elderly people in ChinaAge Ageing20033220521012615566JuanDZhouDHLiJWangJYGaoCChenMA 2-year follow-up study of cigarette smoking and risk of dementiaEur J Neurol20041127728215061830OttAAndersenKDeweyMELetenneurLBrayneCCopelandJRDartiguesJFKragh-SorensenPLoboAMartinez-LageJMStijnenTHofmanALaunerLJEURODEM Incidence Research GroupEffect of smoking on global cognitive function in nondemented elderlyNeurology20046292092415037693SolfrizziVPanzaFColaciccoAMD'IntronoACapursoCTorresFGrigolettoFMaggiSDel ParigiAReimanEMCaselliRJScafatoEFarchiGCapursoAItalian Longitudinal Study on Aging Working GroupVascular risk factors, incidence of MCI, and rates of progression to dementiaNeurology2004631882189115557506AlemanAMullerMde HaanEHvan der SchouwYTVascular risk factors and cognitive function in a sample of independently living menNeurobiol Aging20052648549015653177ReitzCLuchsingerJTangMXMayeuxREffect of smoking and time on cognitive function in the elderly without dementiaNeurology20056587087516186526WhitmerRASidneySSelbyJJohnstonSCYaffeKMidlife cardiovascular risk factors and risk of dementia in late lifeNeurology20056427728115668425StewartMCDearyIJFowkesFGPriceJFRelationship between lifetime smoking, smoking status at older age and human cognitive functionNeuroepidemiology200626839216352911ReitzCden HeijerTvan DuijnCHofmanABretelerMMRelation between smoking and risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease: the Rotterdam studyNeurology200769998100517785668NooyensACvan GelderBMVerschurenWMSmoking and cognitive decline among middle-aged men and women: the Doetinchem cohort studyAm J Public Health2008982244225018923116SabiaSMarmotMDufouilCSingh-ManouxASmoking history and cognitive function in middle age from the Whitehall II studyArch Intern Med20081681165117318541824AlonsoAMosleyTHJrGottesmanRFCatellierDSharrettARCoreshJRisk of dementia hospitalisation associated with cardiovascular risk factors in midlife and older age: the Atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) studyJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry2009801194120119692426CollinsNSachs-EricssonNPreacherKJSheffieldKMMarkidesKSmoking increases risk for cognitive decline among community-dwelling older Mexican AmericansAm J Geriatr Psychiatry20091793494220104052HuangCQDongBRZhangYLWuHMLiuQXAssociation of cognitive impairment with smoking, alcohol consumption, tea consumption, and exercise among Chinese nonagenarians/centenariansCogn Behav Neurol20092219019619741330RusanenMRovioSNganduTNissinenATuomilehtoJSoininenHKivipeltoMMidlife smoking, apolipoprotein E and risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease: a population-based cardiovascular risk factors, aging and dementia studyDement Geriatr Cogn Disord20103027728420847559WangCCLuTHLiaoWCYuanSCKuoPCChuangHLLeeMCYenCHCigarette smoking and cognitive impairment: a 10-year cohort study in TaiwanArch Gerontol Geriatr20105114314819833398RonnemaaEZetheliusBLannfeltLKilanderLVascular risk factors and dementia: 40-year follow-up of a population-based cohortDement Geriatr Cogn Disord20113146046621791923RusanenMKivipeltoMQuesenberryCPJrZhouJWhitmerRAHeavy smoking in midlife and long-term risk of Alzheimer disease and vascular dementiaArch Intern Med201117133333920975015SabiaSElbazADugravotAHeadJShipleyMHagger-JohnsonGKivimakiMSingh-ManouxAImpact of smoking on cognitive decline in early old age: the Whitehall II cohort studyArch Gen Psychiatry20126962763522309970HendrieHCGaoSHallKSHuiSLUnverzagtFWThe relationship between alcohol consumption, cognitive performance, and daily functioning in an urban sample of older black AmericansJ Am Geriatr Soc199644115811658855993DufouilCDucimetierePAlperovitchASex differences in the association between alcohol consumption and cognitive performance. EVA study group. Epidemiology of vascular agingAm J Epidemiol19971464054129290500EliasPKEliasMFD'AgostinoRBSilbershatzHWolfPAAlcohol consumption and cognitive performance in the Framingham heart studyAm J Epidemiol199915058058910489997BondGEBurrRMcCurrySMGravesABLarsonEBAlcohol, aging, and cognitive performance in a cohort of Japanese Americans aged 65 and older: the Kame projectInt Psychogeriatr20011320722311495395ZuccalaGOnderGPedoneCCesariMLandiFBernabeiRCocchiADose-related impact of alcohol consumption on cognitive function in advanced age: results of a multicenter surveyAlcohol Clin Exp Res2001251743174811781507LeroiISheppardJMLyketsosCGCognitive function after 11.5 years of alcohol use: relation to alcohol useAm J Epidemiol200215674775212370163KrahnDFreeseJHauserRBarryKGoodmanBAlcohol use and cognition at mid-life: the importance of adjusting for baseline cognitive ability and educational attainmentAlcohol Clin Exp Res2003271162116612878923AnttilaTHelkalaELViitanenMKareholtIFratiglioniLWinbladBSoininenHTuomilehtoJNissinenAKivipeltoMAlcohol drinking in middle age and subsequent risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in old age: a prospective population based studyBMJ200432953915304383BondGEBurrRMcCurrySMRiceMMBorensteinARKukullWATeriLBowenJDMcCormickWCLarsonEBAlcohol, gender, and cognitive performance: a longitudinal study comparing older Japanese and non-Hispanic white AmericansJ Aging Health20041661564015448275BrittonASingh-ManouxAMarmotMAlcohol consumption and cognitive function in the Whitehall II studyAm J Epidemiol200416024024715257997BondGEBurrRLMcCurrySMRiceMMBorensteinARLarsonEBAlcohol and cognitive performance: a longitudinal study of older Japanese Americans. The Kame projectInt Psychogeriatr20051765366816185373GanguliMVander BiltJSaxtonJAShenCDodgeHHAlcohol consumption and cognitive function in late life: a longitudinal community studyNeurology2005651210121716247047RichardsMHardyRWadsworthMEAlcohol consumption and midlife cognitive change in the British 1946 birth cohort studyAlcohol Alcohol20054011211715582985RodgersBWindsorTDAnsteyKJDearKBFJAChristensenHNon-linear relationships between cognitive function and alcohol consumption in young, middle-aged and older adults: the PATH through life projectAddiction20051001280129016128717StampferMJKangJHChenJCherryRGrodsteinFEffects of moderate alcohol consumption on cognitive function in womenN Engl J Med200535224525315659724ReidMCVan NessPHHawkinsKATowleVConcatoJGuoZLight to moderate alcohol consumption is associated with better cognitive function among older male veterans receiving primary careJ Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol2006199810516690995WrightCBElkindMSLuoXPaikMCSaccoRLReported alcohol consumption and cognitive decline: the northern Manhattan studyNeuroepidemiology20062720120717047373NganduTHelkalaELSoininenHWinbladBTuomilehtoJNissinenAKivipeltoMAlcohol drinking and cognitive functions: findings from the cardiovascular risk factors aging and dementia (CAIDE) studyDement Geriatr Cogn Disord20072314014917170526SolfrizziVD'IntronoAColaciccoAMCapursoCDel ParigiABaldassarreGScapicchioPScafatoEAmodioMCapursoAPanzaFAlcohol consumption, mild cognitive impairment, and progression to dementiaNeurology2007681790179917515541XuGLiuXYinQZhuWZhangRFanXAlcohol consumption and transition of mild cognitive impairment to dementiaPsychiatry Clin Neurosci200963434919154211Au YeungSLJiangCZhangWLamTHChengKKLeungGMSchoolingCMModerate alcohol use and cognitive function in the Guangzhou Biobank cohort studyAnn Epidemiol20102087388220705480ChanKKChiuKCChuLWAssociation between alcohol consumption and cognitive impairment in Southern Chinese older adultsInt J Geriatr Psychiatry2010251272127921086537CorleyJJiaXBrettCEGowAJStarrJMKyleJAMcNeillGDearyIJAlcohol intake and cognitive abilities in old age: the Lothian birth cohort 1936 studyNeuropsychology20112516617521381824GrossALRebokGWFordDEChuAYGalloJJLiangKYMeoniLAShihabHMWangNYKlagMJAlcohol consumption and domain-specific cognitive function in older adults: longitudinal data from the Johns Hopkins precursors studyJ Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci201166394720937708SabiaSGueguenABerrCBerkmanLAnkriJGoldbergMZinsMSingh-ManouxAHigh alcohol consumption in middle-aged adults is associated with poorer cognitive performance only in the low socio-economic group. Results from the GAZEL cohort studyAddiction20111069310120840170ZanjaniFDownerBGKrugerTMWillisSLSchaieKWAlcohol effects on cognitive change in middle-aged and older adultsAging Ment Health2013171122322934837HoSCWooJShamAChanSGYuALA 3-year follow-up study of social, lifestyle and health predictors of cognitive impairment in a Chinese older cohortInt J Epidemiol2001301389139611821352LaurinDVerreaultRLindsayJMacPhersonKRockwoodKPhysical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly personsArch Neurol20015849850411255456SchuitAJFeskensEJLaunerLJKromhoutDPhysical activity and cognitive decline, the role of the apolipoprotein e4 alleleMed Sci Sports Exerc20013377277711323547YaffeKBarnesDNevittMLuiLYCovinskyKA prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walkArch Intern Med20011611703170811485502VergheseJLiptonRBKatzMJHallCBDerbyCAKuslanskyGAmbroseAFSliwinskiMBuschkeHLeisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderlyN Engl J Med20033482508251612815136AbbottRDWhiteLRRossGWMasakiKHCurbJDPetrovitchHWalking and dementia in physically capable elderly menJAMA20042921447145315383515LytleMEVander BiltJPandavRSDodgeHHGanguliMExercise level and cognitive decline: the MoVIES projectAlzheimer Dis Assoc Disord200418576415249848van GelderBMTijhuisMAKalmijnSGiampaoliSNissinenAKromhoutDPhysical activity in relation to cognitive decline in elderly men: the FINE StudyNeurology2004632316232115623693WeuveJKangJHMansonJEBretelerMMWareJHGrodsteinFPhysical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older womenJAMA20042921454146115383516PodewilsLJGuallarEKullerLHFriedLPLopezOLCarlsonMLyketsosCGPhysical activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: findings from the cardiovascular health cognition studyAm J Epidemiol200516163965115781953RovioSKareholtIHelkalaELViitanenMWinbladBTuomilehtoJSoininenHNissinenAKivipeltoMLeisure-time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer's diseaseLancet Neurol2005470571116239176SturmanMTMorrisMCde Leon CFMBieniasJLWilsonRSEvansDAPhysical activity, cognitive activity, and cognitive decline in a biracial community populationArch Neurol2005621750175416286550KarpAPaillard-BorgSWangHXSilversteinMWinbladBFratiglioniLMental, physical and social components in leisure activities equally contribute to decrease dementia riskDement Geriatr Cogn Disord200621657316319455LarsonEBWangLBowenJDMcCormickWCTeriLCranePKukullWExercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and olderAnn Intern Med2006144738116418406LandiFRussoABarillaroCCesariMPahorMDanesePBernabeiROnderGPhysical activity and risk of cognitive impairment among older persons living in the communityAging Clin Exp Res20071941041618007121PsaltopoulouTKyrozisAStathopoulosPTrichopoulosDVassilopoulosDTrichopoulouADiet, physical activity and cognitive impairment among elders: the EPIC-Greece cohort (European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition)Public Health Nutr2008111054106218205988TaaffeDRIrieFMasakiKHAbbottRDPetrovitchHRossGWWhiteLRPhysical activity, physical function, and incident dementia in elderly men: the Honolulu-Asia aging studyJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci20086352953518511759GallucciMAntuonoPOngaroFForloniPLAlbaniDAmiciGPReginiCPhysical activity, socialization and reading in the elderly over the age of seventy: what is the relation with cognitive decline? Evidence from "The Treviso Longeva (TRELONG) study"Arch Gerontol Geriatr20094828428618374429ScarmeasNLuchsingerJASchupfNBrickmanAMCosentinoSTangMXSternYPhysical activity, diet, and risk of Alzheimer diseaseJAMA200930262763719671904YaffeKFioccoAJLindquistKVittinghoffESimonsickEMNewmanABSatterfieldSRosanoCRubinSMAyonayonHNHarrisTBPredictors of maintaining cognitive function in older adults: the Health ABC studyNeurology2009722029203519506226ChangMJonssonPVSnaedalJBjornssonSSaczynskiJSAspelundTEiriksdottirGJonsdottirMKLopezOLHarrisTBGudnasonVLaunerLJThe effect of midlife physical activity on cognitive function among older adults: AGES–Reykjavik studyJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci2010651369137420805238EtgenTSanderDHuntgeburthUPoppertHForstlHBickelHPhysical activity and incident cognitive impairment in elderly persons: the INVADE studyArch Intern Med201017018619320101014GillumRFObisesanTOPhysical activity, cognitive function, and mortality in a US national cohortAnn Epidemiol20102025125720171901MiddletonLEBarnesDELuiLYYaffeKPhysical activity over the life course and its association with cognitive performance and impairment in old ageJ Am Geriatr Soc2010581322132620609030BuchmanASBoylePAYuLShahRCWilsonRSBennettDATotal daily physical activity and the risk of AD and cognitive decline in older adultsNeurology2012781323132922517108LinFFriedmanEQuinnJChenDGMapstoneMEffect of leisure activities on inflammation and cognitive function in an aging sampleArch Gerontol Geriatr201254e39840422377120NortonMCDewJSmithHFauthEPiercyKWBreitnerJCTschanzJWengreenHWelsh-BohmerKLifestyle behavior pattern is associated with different levels of risk for incident dementia and Alzheimer's disease: the Cache county studyJ Am Geriatr Soc20126040541222316091JarvisMJDoes caffeine intake enhance absolute levels of cognitive performance?Psychopharmacology (Berl)199311045527870897Johnson-KozlowMKritz-SilversteinDBarrett-ConnorEMortonDCoffee consumption and cognitive function among older adultsAm J Epidemiol200215684285012397002van BoxtelMPSchmittJABosmaHJollesJThe effects of habitual caffeine use on cognitive change: a longitudinal perspectivePharmacol Biochem Behav20037592192712957237KuriyamaSHozawaAOhmoriKShimazuTMatsuiTEbiharaSAwataSNagatomiRAraiHTsujiIGreen tea consumption and cognitive function: a cross-sectional study from the Tsurugaya Project 1Am J Clin Nutr20068335536116469995van GelderBMBuijsseBTijhuisMKalmijnSGiampaoliSNissinenAKromhoutDCoffee consumption is inversely associated with cognitive decline in elderly European men: the FINE StudyEur J Clin Nutr20076122623216929246NgTPFengLNitiMKuaEHYapKBTea consumption and cognitive impairment and decline in older Chinese adultsAm J Clin Nutr20088822423118614745EskelinenMHNganduTTuomilehtoJSoininenHKivipeltoMMidlife coffee and tea drinking and the risk of late-life dementia: a population-based CAIDE studyJ Alzheimers Dis200916859119158424LaitalaVSKaprioJKoskenvuoMRaihaIRinneJOSilventoinenKCoffee drinking in middle age is not associated with cognitive performance in old ageAm J Clin Nutr20099064064619587088NurkERefsumHDrevonCATellGSNygaardHAEngedalKSmithADIntake of flavonoid-rich wine, tea, and chocolate by elderly men and women is associated with better cognitive test performanceJ Nutr200913912012719056649SmithAPCaffeine, cognitive failures and health in a non-working community sampleHum Psychopharmacol200924293419016251CorleyJJiaXKyleJAGowAJBrettCEStarrJMMcNeillGDearyIJCaffeine consumption and cognitive function at age 70: the Lothian birth cohort 1936 studyPsychosom Med20107220621419995882FengLGweeXKuaEHNgTPCognitive function and tea consumption in community dwelling older Chinese in SingaporeJ Nutr Health Aging20101443343820617284RitchieKArteroSPortetFBrickmanAMuraskinJBeaninoEAncelinMLCarriereICaffeine, cognitive functioning, and white matter lesions in the elderly: establishing causality from epidemiological evidenceJ Alzheimers Dis201020Suppl 1S16116620164564SantosCLunetNAzevedoAde MendoncaARitchieKBarrosHCaffeine intake is associated with a lower risk of cognitive decline: a cohort study from PortugalJ Alzheimers Dis201020Suppl 1S17518520182036ArabLBiggsMLO'MearaESLongstrethWTCranePKFitzpatrickALGender differences in tea, coffee, and cognitive decline in the elderly: the cardiovascular health studyJ Alzheimers Dis20112755356621841254GelberRPPetrovitchHMasakiKHRossGWWhiteLRCoffee intake in midlife and risk of dementia and its neuropathologic correlatesJ Alzheimers Dis20112360761521157028FengLLiJNgTPLeeTSKuaEHZengYTea drinking and cognitive function in oldest-old chineseJ Nutr Health Aging20121675475823131816JamaJWLaunerLJWittemanJCden BreeijenJHBretelerMMGrobbeeDEHofmanADietary antioxidants and cognitive function in a population-based sample of older persons. The Rotterdam StudyAm J Epidemiol19961442752808686696MendelsohnABBelleSHStoehrGPGanguliM Use of antioxidant supplements and its association with cognitive function in a rural elderly cohort: the MoVIES project. Monongahela Valley independent elders surveyAm J Epidemiol199814838449663402MorrisMCBeckettLAScherrPAHebertLEBennettDAFieldTSEvansDAVitamin E and vitamin C supplement use and risk of incident Alzheimer diseaseAlzheimer Dis Assoc Disord1998121211269772012SchmidtRHaynMReinhartBRoobGSchmidtHSchumacherMWatzingerNLaunerLJPlasma antioxidants and cognitive performance in middle-aged and older adults: results of the Austrian stroke prevention studyJ Am Geriatr Soc199846140714109809763MasakiKHLosonczyKGIzmirlianGFoleyDJRossGWPetrovitchHHavlikRWhiteLRAssociation of vitamin E and C supplement use with cognitive function and dementia in elderly menNeurology2000541265127210746596PeacockJMFolsomARKnopmanDSMosleyTHGoffDCJrSzkloMDietary antioxidant intake and cognitive performance in middle-aged adults. The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study investigatorsPublic Health Nutr2000333734310980106EngelhartMJGeerlingsMIRuitenbergAvan SwietenJCHofmanAWittemanJCBretelerMMDietary intake of antioxidants and risk of Alzheimer diseaseJAMA20022873223322912076218MorrisMCEvansDABieniasJLTangneyCCBennettDAAggarwalNWilsonRSScherrPADietary intake of antioxidant nutrients and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a biracial community studyJAMA20022873230323712076219MorrisMCEvansDABieniasJLTangneyCCWilsonRSVitamin E and cognitive decline in older personsArch Neurol2002591125113212117360GraySLHanlonJTLandermanLRArtzMSchmaderKEFillenbaumGGIs antioxidant use protective of cognitive function in the community-dwelling elderly?Am J Geriatr Pharmacother2003131015555461GrodsteinFChenJWillettWCHigh-dose antioxidant supplements and cognitive function in community-dwelling elderly womenAm J Clin Nutr20037797598412663300LuchsingerJATangMXSheaSMayeuxRAntioxidant vitamin intake and risk of Alzheimer diseaseArch Neurol20036020320812580704LaurinDMasakiKHFoleyDJWhiteLRLaunerLJMidlife dietary intake of antioxidants and risk of late-life incident dementia: the Honolulu-Asia aging studyAm J Epidemiol200415995996715128608ZandiPPAnthonyJCKhachaturianASStoneSVGustafsonDTschanzJTNortonMCWelsh-BohmerKABreitnerJCReduced risk of Alzheimer disease in users of antioxidant vitamin supplements: the Cache county studyArch Neurol200461828814732624CherubiniAMartinAAndres-LacuevaCDi IorioALamponiMMecocciPBartaliBCorsiASeninUFerrucciLVitamin E levels, cognitive impairment and dementia in older persons: the InCHIANTI studyNeurobiol Aging20052698799415748776EngelhartMJRuitenbergAMeijerJKiliaanAvan SwietenJCHofmanAWittemanJCBretelerMMPlasma levels of antioxidants are not associated with Alzheimer's disease or cognitive declineDement Geriatr Cogn Disord20051913413915627760FillenbaumGGKuchibhatlaMNHanlonJTArtzMBPieperCFSchmaderKEDyskenMWGraySLDementia and Alzheimer's disease in community-dwelling elders taking vitamin C and/or vitamin EAnn Pharmacother2005392009201416227448MaxwellCJHicksMSHoganDBBasranJEblyEMSupplemental use of antioxidant vitamins and subsequent risk of cognitive decline and dementiaDement Geriatr Cogn Disord200520455115832036MorrisMCEvansDATangneyCCBieniasJLWilsonRSAggarwalNTScherrPARelation of the tocopherol forms to incident Alzheimer disease and to cognitive changeAm J Clin Nutr20058150851415699242AkbaralyNTFaureHGourletVFavierABerrCPlasma carotenoid levels and cognitive performance in an elderly population: results of the EVA studyJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci20076230831617389729DunnJEWeintraubSStoddardAMBanksSSerum alpha-tocopherol, concurrent and past vitamin E intake, and mild cognitive impairmentNeurology20076867067617325274WengreenHJMungerRGCorcoranCDZandiPHaydenKMFotuhiMSkoogINortonMCTschanzJBreitnerJCWelsh-BohmerKAAntioxidant intake and cognitive function of elderly men and women: the Cache county studyJ Nutr Health Aging20071123023717508099FotuhiMZandiPPHaydenKMKhachaturianASSzekelyCAWengreenHMungerRGNortonMCTschanzJTLyketsosCGBreitnerJCWelsh-BohmerKBetter cognitive performance in elderly taking antioxidant vitamins E and C supplements in combination with nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs: the Cache county studyAlzheimers Dement2008422322718631971GraySLAndersonMLCranePKBreitnerJCMcCormickWBowenJDTeriLLarsonEAntioxidant vitamin supplement use and risk of dementia or Alzheimer's disease in older adultsJ Am Geriatr Soc20085629129518047492RavagliaGFortiPLucicesareAPisacaneNRiettiEMangialascheFCecchettiRPattersonCMecocciPPlasma tocopherols and risk of cognitive impairment in an elderly Italian cohortAm J Clin Nutr2008871306131318469254DevoreEEKangJHStampferMJGrodsteinFTotal antioxidant capacity of diet in relation to cognitive function and declineAm J Clin Nutr2010921157116420826624McNeillGJiaXWhalleyLJFoxHCCorleyJGowAJBrettCEStarrJMDearyIJAntioxidant and B vitamin intake in relation to cognitive function in later life in the Lothian birth cohort 1936Eur J Clin Nutr20116561962621346712KalmijnSLaunerLJLindemansJBotsMLHofmanABretelerMMTotal homocysteine and cognitive decline in a community-based sample of elderly subjects: the Rotterdam studyAm J Epidemiol199915028328910430233DuthieSJWhalleyLJCollinsARLeaperSBergerKDearyIJHomocysteine, B vitamin status, and cognitive function in the elderlyAm J Clin Nutr20027590891311976166PrinsNDDen HeijerTHofmanAKoudstaalPJJollesJClarkeRBretelerMMHomocysteine and cognitive function in the elderly: the Rotterdam scan studyNeurology2002591375138012427887SeshadriSBeiserASelhubJJacquesPFRosenbergIHD'AgostinoRBWilsonPWWolfPAPlasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementia and Alzheimer's diseaseN Engl J Med200234647648311844848MillerJWGreenRRamosMIAllenLHMungasDMJagustWJHaanMNHomocysteine and cognitive function in the Sacramento area Latino study on agingAm J Clin Nutr20037844144712936927RavagliaGFortiPMaioliFMuscariASacchettiLArnoneGNativioVTalericoTMarianiEHomocysteine and cognitive function in healthy elderly community dwellers in ItalyAm J Clin Nutr20037766867312600859LuchsingerJATangMXSheaSMillerJGreenRMayeuxRPlasma homocysteine levels and risk of Alzheimer diseaseNeurology2004621972197615184599AriogulSCankurtaranMDagliNKhalilMYavuzBVitamin B12, folate, homocysteine and dementia: are they really related?Arch Gerontol Geriatr20054013914615680498CampbellAKJagustWJMungasDMMillerJWGreenRHaanMNAllenLHLow erythrocyte folate, but not plasma vitamin B-12 or homocysteine, is associated with dementia in elderly LatinosJ Nutr Health Aging20059394315750664EliasMFSullivanLMD'AgostinoRBEliasPKJacquesPFSelhubJSeshadriSAuRBeiserAWolfPAHomocysteine and cognitive performance in the Framingham offspring study: age is importantAm J Epidemiol200516264465316107567KadoDMKarlamanglaASHuangMHTroenARoweJWSelhubJSeemanTEHomocysteine versus the vitamins folate, B6, and B12 as predictors of cognitive function and decline in older high-functioning adults: MacArthur studies of successful agingAm J Med200511816116715694902MooijaartSPGusseklooJFrolichMJollesJStottDJWestendorpRGde CraenAJHomocysteine, vitamin B-12, and folic acid and the risk of cognitive decline in old age: the Leiden 85-Plus studyAm J Clin Nutr20058286687116210718QuadriPFragiacomoCPezzatiRZandaETettamantiMLuccaUHomocysteine and B vitamins in mild cognitive impairment and dementiaClin Chem Lab Med2005431096110016197304RamosMIAllenLHMungasDMJagustWJHaanMNGreenRMillerJWLow folate status is associated with impaired cognitive function and dementia in the Sacramento area Latino study on agingAm J Clin Nutr2005821346135216332669RavagliaGFortiPMaioliFMartelliMServadeiLBrunettiNPorcelliniELicastroFHomocysteine and folate as risk factors for dementia and Alzheimer diseaseAm J Clin Nutr20058263664316155278SchaferJHGlassTABollaKIMintzMJedlickaAESchwartzBSHomocysteine and cognitive function in a population-based study of older adultsJ Am Geriatr Soc20055338138815743278TuckerKLQiaoNScottTRosenbergISpiroA3rdHigh homocysteine and low B vitamins predict cognitive decline in aging men: the Veterans affairs normative aging studyAm J Clin Nutr20058262763516155277EliasMFRobbinsMABudgeMMEliasPKBrennanSLJohnstonCNagyZBatesCJHomocysteine, folate, and vitamins B6 and B12 blood levels in relation to cognitive performance: the Maine-Syracuse studyPsychosom Med20066854755416868263FengLNgTPChuahLNitiMKuaEHHomocysteine, folate, and vitamin B-12 and cognitive performance in older Chinese adults: findings from the Singapore longitudinal ageing studyAm J Clin Nutr2006841506151217158436van RaamtAFKalmijnSMaliWPvan ZandvoortMJvan der GraafYHomocysteine level and cognitive function in patients with arterial disease: the second manifestations of ARTerial disease studyJ Am Geriatr Soc20065457557916686865ClarkeRBirksJNexoEUelandPMSchneedeJScottJMolloyAEvansJGLow vitamin B-12 status and risk of cognitive decline in older adultsAm J Clin Nutr2007861384139117991650HaanMNMillerJWAielloAEWhitmerRAJagustWJMungasDMAllenLHGreenRHomocysteine, B vitamins, and the incidence of dementia and cognitive impairment: results from the Sacramento area Latino study on agingAm J Clin Nutr20078551151717284751KimJParkMHKimEHanCJoSAJoIPlasma homocysteine is associated with the risk of mild cognitive impairment in an elderly Korean populationJ Nutr20071372093209717709448EliasMFRobbinsMABudgeMMEliasPKDoreGABrennanSLJohnstonCNagyZHomocysteine and cognitive performance: modification by the ApoE genotypeNeurosci Lett2008430646918023533KimJMKimSWShinISYangSJParkWYKimSJShinHYYoonJSFolate, vitamin b(12), and homocysteine as risk factors for cognitive decline in the elderlyPsychiatry Investig200853640KimJMStewartRKimSWShinISYangSJShinHYYoonJSChanges in folate, vitamin B12 and homocysteine associated with incident dementiaJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry20087986486818252751ReitzCTangMXMillerJGreenRLuchsingerJAPlasma homocysteine and risk of mild cognitive impairmentDement Geriatr Cogn Disord200927111719088473RedeenSRybergAPeterssonFErikssonONaggaKBorchKHomocysteine levels in chronic gastritis and other conditions: relations to incident cardiovascular disease and dementiaDig Dis Sci20105535135819267198van den KommerTNDikMGComijsHCJonkerCDeegDJHomocysteine and inflammation: predictors of cognitive decline in older persons?Neurobiol Aging2010311700170919004529PerneczkyRAlexopoulosPKurzABickelHCognitive reserve, homocysteine, and cognition in the Bavarian school sisters studyJ Am Geriatr Soc2011591754175622136539ZylbersteinDELissnerLBjorkelundCMehligKThelleDSGustafsonDOstlingSWaernMGuoXSkoogIMidlife homocysteine and late-life dementia in women. A prospective population studyNeurobiol Aging20113238038619342123FordAHFlickerLAlfonsoHHankeyGJNormanPEvan BockxmeerFMAlmeidaOPPlasma homocysteine and MTHFRC677T polymorphism as risk factors for incident dementiaJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry201283707521746742FordAHFlickerLHankeyGJNormanPvan BockxmeerFMAlmeidaOPHomocysteine, methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase C677T polymorphism and cognitive impairment: the health in men studyMol Psychiatry20121755956621358708KalmijnSFeskensEJLaunerLJKromhoutDPolyunsaturated fatty acids, antioxidants, and cognitive function in very old menAm J Epidemiol199714533418982020KalmijnSLaunerLJOttAWittemanJCHofmanABretelerMMDietary fat intake and the risk of incident dementia in the Rotterdam studyAnn Neurol1997427767829392577EngelhartMJGeerlingsMIRuitenbergAVan SwietenJCHofmanAWittemanJCBretelerMMDiet and risk of dementia: does fat matter?: the Rotterdam studyNeurology2002591915192112499483MorrisMCEvansDABieniasJLTangneyCCBennettDAWilsonRSAggarwalNSchneiderJConsumption of fish and n-3 fatty acids and risk of incident Alzheimer diseaseArch Neurol20036094094612873849KalmijnSvan BoxtelMPOckeMVerschurenWMKromhoutDLaunerLJDietary intake of fatty acids and fish in relation to cognitive performance at middle ageNeurology20046227528014745067MorrisMCEvansDATangneyCCBieniasJLWilsonRSFish consumption and cognitive decline with age in a large community studyArch Neurol2005621218495316216930Barberger-GateauPRaffaitinCLetenneurLBerrCTzourioCDartiguesJFAlperovitchADietary patterns and risk of dementia: the Three-City cohort studyNeurology2007691921193017998483BeydounMAKaufmanJSSatiaJARosamondWFolsomARPlasma n-3 fatty acids and risk of cognitive decline among older adults: the atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) studyAm J Clin Nutr2007844110311In press17413112CherubiniAAndres-LacuevaCMartinALauretaniFIorioADBartaliBCorsiABandinelliSMattsonMPFerrucciLLow plasma N-3 fatty acids and dementia in older persons: the InCHIANTI studyJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci2007621120112617921425DullemeijerCDurgaJBrouwerIAvan de RestOKokFJBrummerRJvan BoxtelMPVerhoefPn 3 fatty acid proportions in plasma and cognitive performance in older adultsAm J Clin Nutr2007861479148517991662BeydounMAKaufmanJSSloanePDHeissGIbrahimJn-3 Fatty acids, hypertension and risk of cognitive decline among older adults in the atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) studyPublic Health Nutr200811172917625029EskelinenMHNganduTHelkalaELTuomilehtoJNissinenASoininenHKivipeltoMFat intake at midlife and cognitive impairment later in life: a population-based CAIDE studyInt J Geriatr Psychiatry20082374174718188871SamieriCFeartCLetenneurLDartiguesJFPeresKAuriacombeSPeuchantEDelcourtCBarberger-GateauPLow plasma eicosapentaenoic acid and depressive symptomatology are independent predictors of dementia riskAm J Clin Nutr20088871472118779288AlbaneseEDangourADUauyRAcostaDGuerraMGuerraSSHuangYJacobKSde RodriguezJLNoriegaLHSalasASosaALSousaRMWilliamsJFerriCPPrinceMJDietary fish and meat intake and dementia in Latin America, China, and India: a 10/66 dementia research group population-based studyAm J Clin Nutr20099039240019553298DevoreEEGrodsteinFvan RooijFJHofmanARosnerBStampferMJWittemanJCBretelerMMDietary intake of fish and omega-3 fatty acids in relation to long-term dementia riskAm J Clin Nutr20099017017619474131KrogerEVerreaultRCarmichaelPHLindsayJJulienPDewaillyEAyottePLaurinDOmega-3 fatty acids and risk of dementia: the Canadian study of health and agingAm J Clin Nutr20099018419219474137van de RestOSpiroA3rdKrall-KayeEGeleijnseJMde GrootLCTuckerKLIntakes of (n-3) fatty acids and fatty fish are not associated with cognitive performance and 6-year cognitive change in men participating in the Veterans affairs normative aging studyJ Nutr20091392329233619828689VercambreMNBoutron-RuaultMCRitchieKClavel-ChapelonFBerrCLong-term association of food and nutrient intakes with cognitive and functional decline: a 13-year follow-up study of elderly French womenBr J Nutr200910241942719203415GonzalezSHuertaJMFernandezSPattersonAMLasherasCThe relationship between dietary lipids and cognitive performance in an elderly populationInt J Food Sci Nutr20106121722520001761MuldoonMFRyanCMSheuLYaoJKConklinSMManuckSBSerum phospholipid docosahexaenonic acid is associated with cognitive functioning during middle adulthoodJ Nutr201014084885320181791GaoQNitiMFengLYapKBNgTPOmega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid supplements and cognitive decline: Singapore longitudinal aging studiesJ Nutr Health Aging201115323521267519Kesse-GuyotEPeneauSFerryMJeandelCHercbergSGalanPThirteen-year prospective study between fish consumption, long-chain n-3 fatty acids intakes and cognitive functionJ Nutr Health Aging20111511512021365164SamieriCFeartCProust-LimaCPeuchantEDartiguesJFAmievaHBarberger-GateauPomega-3 fatty acids and cognitive decline: modulation by ApoEepsilon4 allele and depressionNeurobiol Aging2011322317e2313-232220570406ThomsonTMEndnote X32010Philadelphia, PA: Thomson ReutersPetittiDBPetitti DBStatistical methods in meat-analysisMeta-analysis. Decision Analysis, and cost-effectiveness analysis20002000New York, NY: Oxford University PressHildebrandtMBenderRGehrmannUBlettnerMCalculating confidence intervals for impact numbersBMC Med Res Methodol200663216836748EggerMSmithGDAltmanDGSystematic Reviews in heatlh care: Meta-analysis in context20012London. UK: the BMJ Publishing GroupEggerMSmithGDSchneiderMMinderCBias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical testBMJ19973156296349310563BeggCBMazumdarMOperating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication biasBiometrics199450108811017786990EggerMDavey SmithGSchneiderMMinderCDavey SmithGSchneiderMMinderCBias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical testBMJ19973156296349310563STATAEditor edStatistics/Data Analysis: Release 11.0Book Statistics/Data Analysis: Release 11.02009City: Stata CorporationAlbertMSHow does education affect cognitive function?Ann Epidemiol1995576787728289WilsonRSBennettDABeckettLAMorrisMCGilleyDWBieniasJLScherrPAEvansDACognitive activity in older persons from a geographically defined populationJ Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci199954P15516010363036GorelickPBSaccoRLSmithDBAlbertsMMustone-AlexanderLRaderDRossJLRapsEOzerMNBrassLMMaloneMEGoldbergSBoossJHanleyDFTooleJFGreengoldNLRhewDCPrevention of a first stroke: a review of guidelines and a multidisciplinary consensus statement from the National Stroke AssociationJAMA19992811112112010188663MarkesberyWRCarneyJMOxidative alterations in Alzheimer's diseaseBrain Pathol199991331469989456Kellar KJ, Wonnacott SNicotinic cholinergic receptors in Alzheimer's disease, in nicotine psychopharmacology: molecular, cellular, and behavioral aspects1990Oxford: Oxford University PressNordbergABiological markers and the cholinergic hypothesis in Alzheimer's diseaseActa Neurol Scand Suppl199213954581414270LetenneurLLarrieuSBarberger-GateauPAlcohol and tobacco consumption as risk factors of dementia: a review of epidemiological studiesBiomed Pharmacother200458959914992790RogersRLMeyerJSMortelKFAfter reaching retirement age physical activity sustains cerebral perfusion and cognitionJ Am Geriatr Soc1990381231282299115DustmanRERuhlingRORussellEMShearerDEBonekatHWShigeokaJWWoodJSBradfordDCAerobic exercise training and improved neuropsychological function of older individualsNeurobiol Aging1984535426738784SpirdusoWWPhysical fitness, aging, and psychomotor speed: a reviewJ Gerontol1980358508657002994Gomez-PinillaFDaoLSoVPhysical exercise induces FGF-2 and its mRNA in the hippocampusBrain Res1997764189295187CotmanCWEngesser-CesarCExercise enhances and protects brain functionExerc Sport Sci Rev200230757911991541PignattiFRozziniRTrabucchiMPhysical activity and cognitive decline in elderly personsArch Intern Med200216236136211822936JamesJECaffeine & health1991London, England: AcademicBattigKBuzziREffect of coffee on the speed of subject-paced information processingNeuropsychobiology1986161261303587571RiedelWHogervorstELebouxRVerheyFvan PraagHJollesJCaffeine attenuates scopolamine-induced memory impairment in humansPsychopharmacology (Berl)19951221581688848531BehlCAmyloid beta-protein toxicity and oxidative stress in Alzheimer's diseaseCell Tissue Res19972904714809369525ChristenYOxidative stress and Alzheimer diseaseAm J Clin Nutr200071621S629S10681270GrundmanMVitamin E and Alzheimer disease: the basis for additional clinical trialsAm J Clin Nutr200071630S636S10681271RefsumHUelandPMNygardOVollsetSEHomocysteine and cardiovascular diseaseAnnu Rev Med19984931629509248GarciaAHaronYPulmanKHuaLFreedmanMIncreases in homocysteine are related to worsening of stroop scores in healthy elderly persons: a prospective follow-up studyJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci2004591323132715699533TeunissenCEBlomAHVan BoxtelMPBosmaHde BruijnCJollesJWautersBASteinbuschHWde VenteJHomocysteine: a marker for cognitive performance? A longitudinal follow-up studyJ Nutr Health Aging2003715315912766792ScottTMTuckerKLBhadeliaABenjaminBPatzSBhadeliaRLiebsonEPriceLLGriffithJRosenbergIFolsteinMFHomocysteine and B vitamins relate to brain volume and white-matter changes in geriatric patients with psychiatric disordersAm J Geriatr Psychiatry20041263163815545331DufouilCAlperovitchADucrosVTzourioCHomocysteine, white matter hyperintensities, and cognition in healthy elderly peopleAnn Neurol20035321422112557288SachdevPSValenzuelaMWangXLLooiJCBrodatyHRelationship between plasma homocysteine levels and brain atrophy in healthy elderly individualsNeurology2002581539154112034795BleichSKornhuberJRelationship between plasma homocysteine levels and brain atrophy in healthy elderly individualsNeurology2003601220author reply 122012682348den HeijerTVermeerSEClarkeROudkerkMKoudstaalPJHofmanABretelerMMHomocysteine and brain atrophy on MRI of non-demented elderlyBrain200312617017512477704BottiglieriTHomocysteine and folate metabolism in depressionProg Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry2005291103111216109454SelhubJJacquesPFWilsonPWRushDRosenbergIHVitamin status and intake as primary determinants of homocysteinemia in an elderly populationJAMA1993270269326988133587LiLCaoDDesmondRRahmanALahJJLeveyAIZamriniECognitive performance and plasma levels of homocysteine, vitamin B12, folate and lipids in patients with Alzheimer diseaseDement Geriatr Cogn Disord20082638439018931498VidalJSDufouilCDucrosVTzourioCHomocysteine, folate and cognition in a large community-based sample of elderly people–the 3C Dijon StudyNeuroepidemiology20083020721418424901KrumanIICulmseeCChanSLKrumanYGuoZPenixLMattsonMPHomocysteine elicits a DNA damage response in neurons that promotes apoptosis and hypersensitivity to excitotoxicityJ Neurosci2000206920692610995836ParsonsRBWaringRHRamsdenDBWilliamsACIn vitro effect of the cysteine metabolites homocysteic acid, homocysteine and cysteic acid upon human neuronal cell linesNeurotoxicology1998195996039745917ArabLBiomarkers of fat and fatty acid intakeJ Nutr2003133Suppl 3925S932S12612178AndreassiMForleoPDi LorioAMasciSAbateGAmerioPEfficacy of gamma-linolenic acid in the treatment of patients with atopic dermatitisJ Int Med Res1997252662749364289CeroliniSKelsoKANobleRCSpeakeBKPizziFCavalchiniLGRelationship between spermatozoan lipid composition and fertility during aging of chickensBiol Reprod1997579769809369160ZhangLThe effects of essential fatty acids preparation in the treatment of intrauterine growth retardationAm J Perinatol1997145355379394162BjerveKSOmega 3 fatty acid deficiency in man: implications for the requirement of alpha-linolenic acid and long-chain omega 3 fatty acidsWorld Rev Nutr Diet1991661331421675825WainwrightPEDietary essential fatty acids and brain function: a developmental perspective on mechanismsProc Nutr Soc200261616912002796HaagMEssential fatty acids and the brainCan J Psychiatry20034819520312728744de WildeMCHogyesEKiliaanAJFarkasTLuitenPGFarkasEDietary fatty acids alter blood pressure, behavior and brain membrane composition of hypertensive ratsBrain Res200398891914519522